Abstract
Pediatric cerebellar tumor survivors may present with spontaneous language impairments following treatment, but the nature of these impairments is still largely unclear. A recent study by Svaldi et al. (Cerebellum. 23:523-44, 2023) found a broad spectrum of spontaneous language impairments irrespective of postoperative cerebellar mutism syndrome (pCMS) diagnosis in long-term pediatric cerebellar tumor survivors. Several patients presented with reduced grammatical or lexical accuracy, but a detailed error analysis was lacking. The present study builds on this study by 1) investigating the error types in spontaneous language across three language processing levels in the same participant group and 2) by evaluating the possible association between pCMS and the processing nature of the language impairments. Spontaneous language was evaluated in 12 long-term survivors of pediatric cerebellar tumors (M(SD) = 4;8(3;8) years), of whom five were diagnosed with pCMS. The proportion of occurrence of each error type was compared between each patient and five matched controls using individual case statistics, reflecting (lexico-)phonological (i.e., phonemic paraphasias), lexical-semantic (e.g., empty speech) and morphosyntactic processing (e.g., verb inflection errors). Each patient showed a significantly higher proportion of at least one of the included error types across all language processing levels. A higher proportion of general-all-purpose verbs and inaccurate verb inflection were the most common errors and respectively reflected lexical-semantic and morphosyntactic processing. Additional language impairments were identified using the error analysis that were not identified with standard language measures and psycholinguistic analysis, suggesting the added diagnostic value of error analyses.
Similar content being viewed by others
Explore related subjects
Discover the latest articles and news from researchers in related subjects, suggested using machine learning.Avoid common mistakes on your manuscript.
Introduction
Because of an increase in long-term survival in children with cerebellar tumors [1], interest has risen in identifying long-term sequelae of the tumor, the tumor treatment and the need for cognitive rehabilitation [2]. Apart from a broad range of motor and cognitive impairments, cerebellar tumor survivors may present with speech (e.g., dysarthria) and language impairments (e.g., agrammatism) [3,4,5]. Although 25% to 35% of children present with postoperative cerebellar mutism syndrome (pCMS), characterized by severe language deficits, this is not a prerequisite for language difficulties [4, 6].
Previous studies describe language disorders in pediatric cerebellar tumor survivors based on formal language measures (e.g., picture naming task; assessing a specific language function) [3, 5], but also in spontaneous language analyses (e.g., storytelling; assessing multiple language functions simultaneously) [5, 7, 8]. Nonetheless, comprehensive reports of strengths and weaknesses at specific levels of language processing are lacking in the literature though these are necessary for targeted language rehabilitation [2, 6]. Recently, Svaldi et al. [7] provided a detailed spontaneous language analysis across four language processing levels (i.e., semantic, lexical, phonological, morphosyntactic) using standard measures and psycholinguistic properties. Impairments were identified across all levels of language processing, irrespective of pCMS diagnosis. Importantly, several patients presented with reduced lexical and/or grammatical accuracy but an error analysis was lacking. Therefore, the present study will build on this work by 1) characterizing the types of (lexico-)phonological, lexical-semantic and morphosyntactic errors in the spontaneous language of pediatric cerebellar tumor survivors and 2) examining the potential association between pCMS and the nature of the language impairments. The participant group is the same as reported by Svaldi et al. [7].
Methods
Participants
The patient group consisted of 12 (five females, seven males) long-term survivors of pediatric cerebellar tumors with a mean age at assessment of 11;3 years (SD = 6;3 years, range = 3–24;2 years). Patients were recruited via the Erasmus Medical Centre/Sophia Children´s Hospital in Rotterdam as part of an earlier study [9] and time since surgery ranged between 11 months and 12;3 years (M(SD) = 4;8(3;8) years). Five patients suffered from pCMS. Five cerebellar tumor survivors were diagnosed with a medulloblastoma, five with a pilocytic astrocytoma, and two with an ependymoma.
Control data were gathered until each patient could be matched in age and gender with five controls resulting in a total of 39 participants with no history of language or learning impairment (M(SD) = 11;1(5;11) years; range = 3;0–24;3 years), including 24 male and 15 female participants. The absence of language impairment or delay was confirmed through formal language tests and questionnaires (see [7]). A detailed description of the inclusion and exclusion criteria of participants can be found in Svaldi et al. [7] and patient characteristics are reported in the Supplementary materials (reprint from Svaldi et al. [7]). See Appendix A for the cognitive status of patients within one year before language assessment.
Procedure
Spontaneous language samples (minimum 300 words) were gathered based on structured conversations and picture descriptions and were transcribed following the methods outlined in Svaldi et al. [7]. Patient data were collected in 2007 in one testing session. Control data were collected in 2020 and 2021 and consisted of two testing sessions since additional language tests were administered in the first session to rule out language impairment.
Data Coding and Analysis
The samples analyzed by Svaldi et al. [7] were coded for the types of (lexico-) phonological, lexical-semantic and morphosyntactic errors produced (see Appendix B for descriptions of the error types). The proportion of each error type was calculated by dividing the number of errors in the whole sample (i.e., conversation + picture descriptions) by the total number of utterances. The error proportions were compared between each patient and their individually matched set of five controls using modified t-tests for single-case statistics [10].
Results
Each patient produced a greater proportion of errors than controls on at least one of the error types. Significant differences occurred across all language processing levels and are indicative of impaired spontaneous language (see Table 1). Findings of increased error occurrence in patients appeared to be more frequent in the pCMS-group (19 significant findings in five patients) than the non-pCMS-group (12 significant findings in seven patients). Descriptive statistics of the proportions of errors produced and results of the individual case statistics can be found in the Supplementary materials. The level of significance was set at α < 0.05.
Three out of 12 patients, of whom two were diagnosed with pCMS, produced (lexico-) phonological errors (i.e., phonemic paraphasias). Lexical-semantic errors were observed in eight patients. The production of general-all-purpose (GAP) verbs was the most common lexical-semantic error for individuals with and without pCMS. Seven out of 12 patients produced morphosyntactic errors and, while keeping in mind small numbers, tended to occur more in the pCMS-group (4/5 in pCMS-group relative to 3/7 in non-pCMS-group). Here, morphological errors in verb inflection were most often observed, particularly in the pCMS-group.
For eight patients, error patterns aligned with the observations of Svaldi et al. [7] based on standard spontaneous language measures and psycholinguistic properties (see Table 1). For three patients (P23, P17, P16) the psycholinguistic analysis detected impairments in lexical-semantics or morphosyntax that were not confirmed by the error analysis. A greater mismatch was observed for (lexico-)phonological processing, where the error analyses did not reveal significant findings in five out of eight patients in whom the psycholinguistic analysis indicated a phonological impairment.
In contrast, four other patients (P2, P6, P8, P24) showed increased lexical-semantic errors, despite no difference to controls in spontaneous language measures included in Svaldi et al. [7]. This was possibly due to limited measurable spontaneous language for two of these patients (P2 and P6), which led to exclusion from some comparisons. For two patients (P6, P23), error analyses revealed additional morphosyntactic difficulties.
Discussion and Conclusion
The findings of this error analysis demonstrated that long-term (lexico-)phonological, lexical-semantic and morphosyntactic impairments may be present in cerebellar tumor survivors, in the presence of normal scores in other cognitive domains. This aligns with earlier studies and supplements previous reports with a more detailed evaluation of language processing abilities [5, 7, 11]. Such comprehensive evaluation seems thus warranted and necessary for targeted language therapy [2].
Lexical-semantic and morphosyntactic errors were most common in our patient group, aligning with earlier reports on difficulties in these language levels [5, 11, 12]. An increased number of GAP verbs was the most common lexical-semantic error and may reflect immature language or word-finding difficulties [13]. Verb inflection mistakes were the most common morphosyntactic errors and have previously been reported in this population [5]. Further research is necessary, however, to confirm these findings which should include more phonological error types (e.g., phonemic distortions, differentiating between programming and selection impairment).
More differences between patients and controls were found in the pCMS-group compared to the non-pCMS-group. Morphosyntactic disorders may be particularly frequent in the pCMS-group [6]. Notably, preoperative morphosyntactic disorders were suggested as risk factors for pCMS [11]. This could mean that the higher proportion of morphosyntactic errors in the pCMS-group reflects pre-surgical impairments. However, this can only be established in future research including a preoperative assessment and equal groups of patients with and without pCMS, as well as a larger sample size so that more generalizable patterns may emerge. Similarly, prevalence data is notoriously difficult to establish in small-N studies.
It should also be considered that the occurrence of pCMS may result in less reliable spontaneous language assessment, given the reduced output. However, where standard and psycholinguistic measures detected no impairments for some patients [7], the error analysis did. The opposite was also observed, indicating that different analyses can provide complementary information, potentially tapping into different underlying impairments. A combination of standard measures, psycholinguistic analyses, and error analyses may increase the sensitivity to identify impairments and to characterize their language processing nature [13].
The error analysis conducted in the present study suggested long-term language impairment in cerebellar tumor survivors. A detailed and individualized language follow-up seems necessary to investigate the processing nature of these impairments.
Data Availability
The patient data analyzed during the current study were shared with us and requests for data sharing should go to Prof Philippe Paquier. Control data are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request and can be shared via DataverseNL.
References
Ramaswamy V, Remke M, Adamski J, Bartels U, Tabori U, Wang X, et al. Medulloblastoma subgroup-specific outcomes in irradiated children: Who are the true high-risk patients? Neuro Oncol. 2016;18:291–7.
Paquier PF, Walsh KS, Docking KM, Hartley H, Kumar R, Catsman-Berrevoets CE. Post-operative cerebellar mutism syndrome: Rehabilitation issues. Child’s Nervous System. 2020;36:1215–22.
Cámara S, Fournier C, Cordero P, Melero J, Robles F, Esteso B, et al. Neuropsychological profile in children with posterior fossa tumors with or without postoperative cerebellar mutism syndrome (CMS). Cerebellum. 2020;19:78–88.
Gudrunardottir T, Morgan AT, Lux AL, Walker DA, Walsh KS, Wells EM, et al. Consensus paper on post-operative pediatric cerebellar mutism syndrome: The Iceland Delphi results. Child’s Nervous Syst. 2016;32:1195–203.
Riva D, Giorgi C. The cerebellum contributes to higher functions during development: Evidence from a series of children surgically treated for posterior fossa tumours. Brain. 2000;123:1051–61.
Svaldi C, Ntemou E, Jonkers R, Kohnen S, de Aguiar V. Language outcomes in children who underwent surgery for the removal of a posterior fossa tumor: A systematic review. Eur J Paediatr Neurol. 2024;48:129–41.
Svaldi C, Paquier P, Keulen S, van Elp H, Catsman-Berrevoets C, Kingma A, et al. Characterising the long-term language impairments of children following cerebellar tumour surgery by extracting psycholinguistic properties from spontaneous language. Cerebellum. 2023;23:523–44.
Docking K, Knijnik SR. Prospective longitudinal decline in cognitive-communication skills following treatment for childhood brain tumor. Brain Inj. 2021;35:1472–9.
De Smet HJ, Catsman-Berrevoets C, Aarsen F, Verhoeven J, Mariën P, Paquier PF. Auditory-perceptual speech analysis in children with cerebellar tumours: a long-term follow-up study. Eur J Paediatr Neurol. 2012;16:434–42.
Crawford JR, Garthwaite PH. Investigation of the single case in neuropsychology: Confidence limits on the abnormality of test scores and test score differences. Neuropsychologia. 2002;40:1196–208.
Di Rocco C, Chieffo D, Frassanito P, Caldarelli M, Massimi L, Tamburrini G. Heralding cerebellar mutism: Evidence for pre-surgical language impairment as primary risk factor in posterior fossa surgery. Cerebellum. 2011;10:551–62.
Persson K, Boeg Thomsen D, Fyrberg Å, Castor C, AasvedHjort M, Andreozzi B, et al. Preoperative word-finding difficulties in children with posterior fossa tumours: A European cross-sectional study. Childs Nerv Syst. 2023;40(1):87–97.
Dunn M, Flax J, Sliwinski M, Aram D. The use of spontaneous language measures as criteria for identifying children with Specific Language Impairment: An attempt to reconcile clinical and research incongruence. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 1996;39:643–54.
Acknowledgements
We thank all the children and their parents for participating in this study, and acknowledge the contribution of Hyo Jung De Smet, PhD, in an early phase of this study.
Funding
This study is part of a doctoral research funded by the International Doctorate in Experimental Approaches to Language and Brain (IDEALAB) via the University of Groningen and Macquarie University. This publication is also part of the project “Verb processing and verb learning in children with pediatric posterior fossa tumors” (with project number VI.Vidi.201.003) of the research programme ‘NWO-Talentprogramma Vidi SGW 2020’ which is financed by the Dutch Research Council (NWO). This publication is also part of the project “Educational success and language impairment in children with paediatric posterior fossa tumours (ESLI-PPFTs)” (with project number 406.XS.04.073) of the research programme ‘NWO Open Competition Domain Social Sciences and Humanities – XS’ which is financed by the Dutch Research Council (NWO).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
All authors contributed to the study conception and design except for Juan-Ignacio Galli. Material provision, data collection and analysis were performed by Cheyenne Svaldi, Coriene Catsman-Berrevoets, Philippe Paquier, Henrieke Van Elp and Juan-Ignacio Galli. The first draft of the manuscript was written by Cheyenne Svaldi and all authors commented on previous versions of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Corresponding authors
Ethics declarations
Ethics Approval
All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. According to Dutch law, no approval of a Medical Ethical Committee is needed when patient data are studied that are obtained as part of routine patient care. Ethical approval for the collection of data from participants without neurological or language impairments was obtained from the Research Ethics Committee (CETO) from the faculty of arts of the University of Groningen (review number 76303408).
Informed Consent
As is required by Dutch law, parents and patients over 12 years old signed a consent form before neuropsychological assessment in which they allowed the data to be used for research.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Material
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Svaldi, C., Galli, JI., Paquier, P. et al. Errors in the Spontaneous Language of Survivors of Pediatric Cerebellar Tumors. Cerebellum 24, 26 (2025). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12311-024-01754-2
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12311-024-01754-2