Abstract
This dataset originates from TeensLab, a consortium of Spanish Universities dedicated to behavioral research involving Spanish teenagers. The dataset contains data from 33 distinct educational institutions across Spain, accounting for a total of 5,890 students aged 10 to 23 (M = 14.10, SD = 1.94), representing various educational levels such as primary school, secondary school, sixth form and vocational training. The main dimensions covered in this dataset include (i) economic preferences, (ii) cognitive abilities and (iii) strategic thinking. Additionally, a range of supplementary variables is included alongside socio-demographic factors, capturing data on aspects like physical appearance, mood and expectations, among others.
Similar content being viewed by others
Background & Summary
Adolescence is a stage of major physical, psychological, emotional and social development, representing a crucial period in human life. The experiences, skills and habits that are accumulated during this stage have a permanent impact on human life. Therefore, understanding the behavior of individuals throughout this period is essential to supporting their development and ensuring their success in adulthood. Indeed, there is great interest in underlying motivations of adolescent behaviors for the design of public policies1.
It is widely recognized that individual preferences and cognitive abilities are important determinants of real-life decision-making of adults in strategic and non-strategic situations2,3,4,5,6. To understand and predict adult behavior, it is essential to comprehend how their attitudes toward risk, social and time preferences, cognitive abilities, creativity, and other traits evolve, particularly in their younger years7,8,9,10,11,12,13,14,15,16,17,18.
The dataset presented here contributes to the literature on adolescence by eliciting-using the tools of experimental economics-rich information on economic preferences, cognitive abilities, strategic thinking behavior and other information from a large set of adolescents in Spain. We conducted lab-in-the-field experiments in 33 different educational centers, accounting for a total of 5,890 observations of Spanish students. The centers belong to 19 different locations. A total of 20 of them are public and the rest are semi-private. In addition to socio-demographic details and other variables related to the individual, the data includes several sets of variables: economic preferences, cognitive abilities, strategic thinking and other additional measures.
Our dataset can contribute to future research on adolescents in at least two ways. First, it allows researchers to study adolescent decision-making and understand developmental causes of anomalous behavior. Second, it provides information on economic preferences, cognitive skills and other individual information, enabling exploring the extent to which these variables are sensitive to the class and school environment.
This dataset has been previously employed in the following studies: (i) An analysis of the relevance of monetary incentives, experimental tools and protocols to collect data in schools19, (ii) a study of the impact of visual aids in experimental lottery tasks to reduce inconsistency among adolescents20, (iii) the development of time and risk preferences throughout the adolescence21, (iv) the dynamics of social preferences among girls and boys22 and (v) the use of coordination devices among adolescents23. These studies as well as information about the TeensLab can be found on our website (https://loyolabehlab.org/teenslab/).
Methods
Data acquisition
In conducting research with minors, adherence to legal frameworks and ethical guidelines is essential. Spanish law governing the protection of personal data of minors allows for data processing based on the consent of children over 14 years of age (Art. 724). Although our study included participants over 14, we obtained informed parental consent through a parental council that had approved the study, enabling the integration of the study into the school curriculum. This consent authorized participation and the anonymous sharing and use of data within the scientific community. In this way, parental consent is collected by the center itself at the beginning of the scholar-year where they present the activities planned, including this experiment. This strategy not only simplified the process but also facilitated scalability.
Participants were informed about the purpose of data processing, the confidentiality of their responses, and the legal framework governing their data. Teachers managed participant lists and assigned identification numbers to ensure confidentiality. All responses were recorded anonymously.
Informed consent was additionally obtained from all participants on the initial screen of the experiment. This mandatory screen provided essential information in compliance with data protection regulations. Table 1 presents a translation of this information, which includes the identity of the data controller and a description of the rights participants may exercise.
Our experiment was approved by the Ethical Committee of Universidad Loyola Andalucía (No. 20190318, 20200709 and 20230301) Furthermore, for 10-year-olds, it was also approved by the Bioethics Commission of the University of Barcelona (No. IRB00003099).
To mitigate issues related to non-standard samples and minimize missing data, we simplified response formats, predominantly using multiple-choice questions rather than open-ended ones. The design of the software required that participants could not skip questions. However, for potentially sensitive topics, they were allowed to choose the option “I would prefer not to answer”.
The participant pool was recruited through agreements with school headmasters, who agreed to integrate the experiment into their pedagogical curriculum and to carry it out as a classroom activity. Consequently, we achieved a high level of participation19. The experiments were conducted on-site at schools using an online platform named Social Analysis and Network Data (SAND; https://sand.kampal.com), enhancing data privacy control. This platform allows students to navigate and complete the experimental questionnaire, which is divided into several sections, on their devices (tablets, computers, or smartphones).
The questionnaire was administered entirely in Spanish. Due to the restrictive school policies on experiments involving real money, we used hypothetical rewards. However, it has been documented that the behavior of adolescents does not differ between incentivized and hypothetical payment schemes at least for risk and time preferences, suggesting the reliability of the findings25,26,27,28,29,30,31.
Measurements
Table 2 contains all the tasks included in the study. Apart from basic information regarding the school (province, city and public/semi-private) and the class (stage, grade, group, class size), our dataset includes individual-level measurements for the following three behavioral dimensions:
-
Economic preferences: Time discounting, involving choices between immediate and delayed rewards (patience)19,32; risk preferences, assessed through decisions involving probabilistic outcomes (prudence)19,20; social preferences, measured via resource allocation tasks (egalitarianism, altruism, spitefulness)33,34,35; and honesty, evaluated through opportunities to misreport outcomes36.
-
Cognitive abilities: Cognitive reflection, overriding intuitive responses19,37; financial abilities, solving simple financial calculations19; probability knowledge and accuracy, measured via decisions in probabilistic scenarios38 and creativity, generating multiple original ideas using a single object39.
-
Strategic thinking: Subjects choices and expectations in strategic environments (games)22.
We also collected information regarding the participant’s family background and outcomes in school:
-
Socio-demographics: Age, gender, self-reported income, migrant status and family composition (number of siblings and her ranking).
-
GPA: The self-reported number of A’s and B’s scored in Mathematics, English and Spanish Literature during the previous year.
-
Physical appearance: Self-reported height, weight and appearance by Stunkard figure scale40,41.
-
Mood: Three items from the Kidscreen questionnaire about their interactions at school, assessing whether they have fun with their friends or feel lonely42,43.
Finally, for certain sub-samples (see available observations in Table 2), we gathered additional auxiliary information:
-
Expectations: Information regarding subjects’ expectations about their future outcomes, such as their university degree, traveling around the world, living abroad and desired future job.
-
Self-assessed math abilities: Two types of questions: “How good are you at maths?” and “How much do you like maths?”44.
-
Time discounting II: Time preferences (patience) measured by the compound staircase version45.
-
Time perception: Questions about future actions at three levels46.
Data Records
The dataset can be found in Zenodo47 (https://zenodo.org/records/13720112) and is available in different formats (xls, cvs, dta). The screenshots of the complete experimental instructions are also available in the repository. We also provide STATA 1848 scripts for some basic summaries of the available variables.
Sample variables
The experiments were conducted over multiple sessions from 2021 to 2023. A total of 5,890 students started the experiment, but 609 did not finish the entire questionnaire.
In contrast to adults, it is well-known that children and adolescents often find it more difficult to maintain concentration over extended periods and to complete all tasks13. Some of them simply leave the survey at a certain point. We check the responses after each session and reassess the tasks which were not successful. As a result of various adjustments made during the experimental sessions, the survey tasks underwent some changes. Consequently, the number of observations for different variables in our dataset varies. Table 2 provides an overview of the available observations for each task.
The initial questionnaire screen (Table 1) provided essential information about the study, including an introduction to our team and funding sources.
Figure 1 displays the distribution of the final sample by age and gender. The sample is well-balanced in terms of gender; 49.68% are female students and 49.68% male. The remaining subjects (0.64%) are classified as unknown, either because they did not answer or they selected another category.
As for educational stages, 8.62% of the sample belongs to primary education, 84.94% belongs to secondary education, 1.90% to sixth form and 4.53% to vocational training. Table 3 presents the distribution of the observations by educational stage. Additionally, it displays the response rate and summary statistics for the ages at each stage.
Technical Validation
The study is a laboratory-in-the-field experiment. Data were collected in the school classrooms under the supervision of team members and research assistants.
The data recorded in the software were downloaded for cleaning using Stata48. Variables were coded and incomplete entries were not deleted. Only the age variable was imputed through the year of birth reported by the students and according to the course to which they belonged.
Our experiment includes standard tasks from the literature as well as tasks adapted by our research team from previous literature19. We have extensive prior experience in designing experiments for teenagers and collecting data in primary and secondary schools using lab-in-the-field techniques. Previous evidence suggests that there are no significant differences in outcomes when using hypothetical payoff tasks, such as eliciting risk preferences27,28,29,30,31. Prior to each task, students were provided with a brief description and they were informed of the economic implications of their decisions in hypothetical terms. This ensured that participants fully understood the nature of the tasks, while maintaining the validity of the experimental design and the scalability of the study. Some pilots of the tasks were carried out independently to configure the final design. The changes in the survey are detailed in the variables descriptor available in the repository.
One of the main problems in collecting data from non-standard samples is that some tasks are not understood and participants show inconsistent behavior across them. To address this, our design took into account the results of pilots that adapted the tasks to the adolescent context through the use of visual aids. As a result, the consistency rates are remarkably higher than those reported in the literature7,19,20.
Consistency is assessed by examining how the choices of participants align with their stated preferences in different situations, based on their personal decision-making patterns. Among the data reported for the economic preferences dimension, we find a high percentage of consistent responses in the tasks that require certain within-task consistency. We observe that 82.75% of the individuals who complete the time preference task exhibit consistent behavior. Similarly, 79.20% of individuals report consistent answers in the risk preferences task20. Table 4 includes a distribution of responses for both tasks across their 6 decisions, where a trend can be identified that may represent this high level of consistency. Such enhanced consistency indicates that the data collected from adolescents are reliable and coherent, providing a robust foundation for examining adolescent decision-making processes and developmental trends.
Usage Notes
The Zenodo repository gives access to the available data together with a descriptive note on the variables and their coding. The variable descriptor includes a definition of the task, some general characteristics, and the specific name under which it is found in the database. We provide further information on the changes that the survey has made over time. In addition, the repository visualizes the experimental screens in the original Spanish language.
This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) 4.0 International License.
Code availability
STATA 1848 software was used. The code for the variables can be found in the aforementioned repository.
References
Dahl, R. E., Allen, N. B., Wilbrecht, L. & Suleiman, A. B. Importance of investing in adolescence from a developmental science perspective. Nature 554, 441–450, https://doi.org/10.1038/nature25770 (2018).
Dohmen, T., Falk, A., Huffman, D. & Sunde, U. Are risk aversion and impatience related to cognitive ability? American Economic Review 100, 1238–1260, https://www.aeaweb.org/articles?id=10.1257/aer.100.3.1238 (2010).
Falk, A. et al. Global evidence on economic preferences. The Quarterly Journal of Economics 133, 1645–1692, https://doi.org/10.1093/qje/qjy013 (2018).
Eckel, C., Johnson, C. & Montmarquette, C. Saving decisions of the working poor: Short-and long-term horizons. In Field Experiments in Economics, 219–260, https://www.emerald.com/insight/content/doi/10.1016/S0193-2306-9 (Emerald Group Publishing Limited, 2005).
Dohmen, T. et al. Individual risk attitudes: Measurement, determinants, and behavioral consequences. Journal of the European Economic Association 9, 522–550, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1542-4774.2011.01015.x (2011).
Chapman, G. B. Time discounting of health outcomes. Economic and Psychological Perspectives on Intertemporal Choice 395–418, https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2003-06265-013 (2003).
Sutter, M., Zoller, C. & Glätzle-Rützler, D. Economic behavior of children and adolescents–a first survey of experimental economics results. European Economic Review 111, 98–121, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroecorev.2018.09.004 (2019).
Angerer, S., Bolvashenkova, J., Glätzle-Rützler, D., Lergetporer, P. & Sutter, M. Children’s patience and school-track choices several years later: Linking experimental and field data. Journal of Public Economics 220, 104837, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpubeco.2023.104837 (2023).
Golsteyn, B. H., Grönqvist, H. & Lindahl, L. Adolescent time preferences predict lifetime outcomes. The Economic Journal 124, F739–F761, https://doi.org/10.1111/ecoj.12095 (2014).
Piovesan, M. & Willadsen, H. Risk preferences and personality traits in children and adolescents. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization 186, 523–532, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2021.04.011 (2021).
Andreoni, J., Di Girolamo, A., List, J. A., Mackevicius, C. & Samek, A. Risk preferences of children and adolescents in relation to gender, cognitive skills, soft skills, and executive functions. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization 179, 729–742, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2019.05.002 (2020).
Brocas, I. & Carrillo, J. D. Introduction to special issue “understanding cognition and decision making by children” studying decision-making in children: Challenges and opportunities. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization 179, 777–783, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2020.01.020 (2020).
List, J. A., Petrie, R. & Samek, A. How experiments with children inform economics. Journal of Economic Literature 61, 504–564, https://www.aeaweb.org/articles?id=10.1257/jel.20211535 (2023).
Wong, C. A. et al. Applying behavioral economics to improve adolescent and young adult health: a developmentally-sensitive approach. Journal of Adolescent Health 69, 17–25, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2020.10.007 (2021).
Brocas, I. & Carrillo, J. D. Steps of reasoning in children and adolescents. Journal of Political Economy 129, 2067–2111, https://doi.org/10.1086/714118 (2021).
Lundberg, S., Romich, J. L. & Tsang, K. P. Decision-making by children. Review of Economics of the Household 7, 1–30, https://doi.org/10.1007/s11150-008-9045-2 (2009).
Samek, A., Gray, A., Datar, A. & Nicosia, N. Adolescent time and risk preferences: Measurement, determinants and field consequences. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization 184, 460–488, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2020.12.023 (2021).
Huurre, T., Aro, H., Rahkonen, O. & Komulainen, E. Health, lifestyle, family and school factors in adolescence: predicting adult educational level. Educational Research 48, 41–53, https://doi.org/10.1080/00131880500498438 (2006).
Alfonso, A. et al. The adventure of running experiments with teenagers. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Economics 102048, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socec.2023.102048 (2023).
Vasco, M. & De Francisco, M. J. V. Holt-laury as a gumball machine. Available at SSRN 4491386 https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.4491386 (2023).
Alfonso, A., Brañas-Garza, P., Jorrat, D., Prissé, B. & Vazquez, M. The baking of preferences throughout high school. Tech. Rep., Mimeo. https://ideas.repec.org/p/aoz/wpaper/316.html (2024).
Brañas-Garza, P. Teenage girls are egalitarian, and boys are generous. Available at SSRN 4730138 https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.4730138 (2024).
Brañas-Garza, P. & Lomas, P. Developmental equilibrium selection. Available at SSRN 4712366 https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.4712366 (2023).
Organic law 3/2018, of december 5, on the protection of personal data and guarantee of digital rights (2018).
Brañas-Garza, P., Estepa-Mohedano, L., Jorrat, D., Orozco, V. & Rascón-Ramírez, E. To pay or not to pay: Measuring risk preferences in lab and field. Judgment and Decision Making 16, 1290–1313, https://doi.org/10.1017/S1930297500008433 (2021).
Brañas-Garza, P., Jorrat, D., Espn, A. M. & Sánchez, A. Paid and hypothetical time preferences are the same: Lab, field and online evidence. Experimental Economics 26, 412–434, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10683-022-09776-5 (2023).
Kühberger, A., Schulte-Mecklenbeck, M. & Perner, J. Framing decisions: Hypothetical and real. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 89, 1162–1175, https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-5978(02)00021-3 (2002).
Chuang, Y. & Schechter, L. Stability of experimental and survey measures of risk, time, and social preferences: A review and some new results. Journal of Development Economics 117, 151–170, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jdeveco.2015.07.008 (2015).
Read, D. Monetary incentives, what are they good for? Journal of Economic Methodology 12, 265–276, https://doi.org/10.1080/13501780500086180 (2005).
Beattie, J. & Loomes, G. The impact of incentives upon risky choice experiments. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 14, 155–168, https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1007721327452 (1997).
Horn, S. & Freund, A. M. Adult age differences in monetary decisions with real and hypothetical reward. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 35, e2253, https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.2253 (2022).
Coller, M. & Williams, M. B. Eliciting individual discount rates. Experimental Economics 2, 107–127, https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1009986005690 (1999).
Fehr, E., Bernhard, H. & Rockenbach, B. Egalitarianism in young children. Nature 454, 1079–1083, https://doi.org/10.1038/nature07155 (2008).
Corgnet, B., Espn, A. M. & Hernán-González, R. The cognitive basis of social behavior: cognitive reflection overrides antisocial but not always prosocial motives. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 9, 287, https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00287 (2015).
Brañas-Garza, P., Cabrales, A., Espinosa, M. P. & Jorrat, D. The effect of ambiguity in strategic environments: an experiment. arXiv preprint https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.2209.11079 (2022).
Fischbacher, U. & Föllmi-Heusi, F. Lies in disguise—an experimental study on cheating. Journal of the European Economic Association 11, 525–547, https://doi.org/10.1111/jeea.12014 (2013).
Thomson, K. S. & Oppenheimer, D. M. Investigating an alternate form of the cognitive reflection test. Judgment and Decision Making 11, 99–113, https://doi.org/10.1017/S1930297500007622 (2016).
Delavande, A. & Kohler, H.-P. Subjective expectations in the context of hiv/aids in malawi. Demographic Research 20, 817, https://doi.org/10.4054/DemRes.2009.20.31 (2009).
Guilford, J. P. Creativity: Yesterday, today and tomorrow. The Journal of Creative Behavior 1, 3–14, https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2162-6057.1967.tb00002.x (1967).
Stunkard, A. J. et al. An adoption study of human obesity. New England Journal of Medicine 314, 193–198, https://www.nejm.org/doi/abs/10.1056/nejm198601233140401 (1986).
Carrasco, R. & Gonzalez, D. The impact of obesity on human capital accumulation: An analysis of driving factors. Mimeo https://documentos.fedea.net/pubs/dt/2024/dt2024-03.pdf (2024).
Aymerich, M. et al. Desarrollo de la versión en español del kidscreen: un cuestionario de calidad de vida para la población infantil y adolescente. Gaceta Sanitaria 19, 93–102, https://www.scielosp.org/pdf/gs/2005.v19n2/93-102/es (2005).
Ravens-Sieberer, U. et al. Kidscreen-52 quality-of-life measure for children and adolescents. Expert Review of Pharmacoeconomics & Outcomes Research 5, 353–364, https://doi.org/10.1586/14737167.5.3.353 (2005).
Adamecz-Völgyi, A., Jerrim, J., Pingault, J.-B. & Shure, D. Overconfident boys: The gender gap in mathematics self-assessment. IZA Discussion Paper https://www.econstor.eu/handle/10419/278878 (2023).
Falk, A., Becker, A., Dohmen, T., Huffman, D. & Sunde, U. The preference survey module: A validated instrument for measuring risk, time, and social preferences. Management Science 69, 1935–1950, https://doi.org/10.1287/mnsc.2022.4455 (2023).
Keller, T., Kiss, H. J. & Szakál, P. Endogenous language use and patience. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization 220, 792–812, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2024.02.013 (2024).
Vasco, M. et al. Teenslab dataset https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.13720112 (2024).
StataCorp. Stata statistical software: Release 18. College Station TX: StataCorp LLC. (2023).
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank Javier Gonzalez for his amazing job as an illustrator and the following RAs who helped to run the experiments in the schools: Laura Giraldo, Juan Gonzalez, Emilio Nieto, Emilio Pericet, Paula Piña, and Luis Rivero. This research was supported by the Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness (PID2022-141802NB-I00 and PID2021-126892NB-I00); the Spanish Ministry of Science, Innovation and Universities (PID2023-147817NB-I0, PID2022-140661OB-I00, FPU20/05846, PID2019-106146GB-I00 and PGC2018- 093506-B-I00); Excelencia-Junta Andalucía (PY-18-FR-0007); the Andalusian Agency for International Development Cooperation (AACID-0I008/2020); the Department of University, Research and Innovation of the Junta de Andalucía and as appropriate, by “ERDF A way of making Europe”, by the European Union (B-SEJ-280-UGR20); the María de Maeztu Programme of Agencia Estatal de Investigación (CEX2021-001181-M); Comunidad de Madrid EPUC3M11 (V PRICIT); the University of the Basque Country (EHU-N23/50) and the Basque Government (IT1461-22).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
A.Al., A.C., J.A.C., A.E., M.P.E., D.J., J.K., D.M., A.S., M.J.V. and P.B. conceived the experiment. M.V., A.Al., A.Ar., T.G., A.I., D.J., P.L., A.C.M., M.P.R., P.R. and M.J.V. conducted the experiment. A.C., J.A.C., J.K., D.M. and P.B. provided data or analysis tools. M.V., A.Al., A.Ar., T.G., D.J., P.L., A.C.M., M.P.R., P.R. and A.S. performed the analysis. M.V., T.G. and J.K. wrote the paper. A.Al., A.C., J.A.C., A.E., M.P.E., D.J., P.L., D.M., A.S. and P.B. reviewed the paper.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License, which permits any non-commercial use, sharing, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if you modified the licensed material. You do not have permission under this licence to share adapted material derived from this article or parts of it. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Vasco, M., Alfonso, A., Arenas, A. et al. Economic preferences and cognitive abilities among teenagers in Spain. Sci Data 12, 7 (2025). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41597-024-04298-6
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41597-024-04298-6