Abstract
Following the Coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) pandemic, a large number of people continue to report Post-COVID symptoms (PCS). A wide variety of symptoms are described, including fatigue, post-exertional malaise and cognitive impairment. However, adequate objective diagnostic tests for PCS are not yet available. Since the neurotropism of SARS-CoV-2 could be a possible factor for cognitive impairment, the aim of this study was to clarify whether visual reaction time (RT) in a stereoscopic setting can be a marker in PCS diagnostics. The Virtual-Reality-Oculomotor-Test-System (VR-OTS) was used testing binocular vision in 9 gaze directions via stereoscopic stimuli displayed in a virtual reality (VR)-environment (disparity: 275″, 550″, 1100″) in 179 individuals: 130 patients with PCS and 49 healthy controls. The results from the generalized linear models indicated that both group membership (PCS vs. control) and covariates (age and sex) yielded statistically significant different RT across the models. Accounting for the effect of covariates a statistically significant difference of RT was observed between patients with PCS and controls (disparity 275″ p-value = 0.001; 550″ p-value = 0.001; 1100″ p-value = 0.003). Patients with PCS performed worse in RT in all gaze directions, respectively. Adjusting for the influence of covariates, correct responses (CR) differed significantly between patients with PCS and controls (disparity 275″ p-value < 0.001; 550″ p-value = 0.003; 1100″ p-value = 0.019). Statistically significant effects of covariates on RT were observed for sex (disparity 275″ p-value = 0.047; 550″ p-value = 0.012; 1100″ p-value = 0.005) and age (disparity 275″ p-value < 0.001; 550″ p-value < 0.001; 1100″ p-value < 0.001). However, regarding covariates, no significant effects were found for CR, except for age at disparity 275″ (p-value = 0.035). The present data suggested that the mentioned variables uniquely contributed to explain the variation of the response variable (RT, CR). RT and CR detecting 3D-stimuli in a virtual 3D- environment might offer novel functional diagnostic approaches in PCS.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
Coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19), a disorder caused by the severe acute respiratory syndrome coronavirus type 2 (SARS-CoV-2) became a global pandemic on March 11, 2020 1. According to the WHO over 762 million confirmed cases of COVID-19, including over 6.8 million deaths were known worldwide until April 2023 2.
Patients were not only suffering from acute COVID-19 symptoms, yet patients report of ongoing symptoms (Post-COVID syndrome, PCS). According to the WHO, “Post COVID-19 condition occurs in individuals with a history of probable or confirmed SARS CoV-2 infection, usually 3 months from the onset of COVID-19 with symptoms and that last for at least 2 months and cannot be explained by an alternative diagnosis”3,4. A global estimated prevalence of 12.7% 5 (ranging from 7.5 to 43% 6,7) in the general population after COVID-19 is estimated. Systematic reviews show a substantial prevalence of PCS7,8, yet data are still highly heterogeneous8. A wide range of PCS symptoms have been reported: in general, fatigue (including post-exertional malaise (PEM)), cardiac (e.g. tachycardia, postural orthostatic tachycardia syndrome, POTS), neurological (e.g. brain fog, sleep disorders), respiratory, gastrointestinal symptoms, and musculoskeletal pain7,8,9,10,11,12. Recent data indicated that 3 to less than 6 months after acute COVID-19, the most commonly reported symptoms were fatigue (including PEM), dyspnea, sleep disorder, and loss of concentration reported at rates of 32%, 25%, 24%, and 22% respectively8. At a follow-up of 6 to less than 9 months the most common symptoms were PEM, fatigue, sleep disorder, and dyspnea reported at rates of 45%, 36%, 29%, and 25% respectively8.
The exact pathogenesis is still elusive. Apparently, PCS seems to be a complex and multifactorial syndrome encompassing several subtypes (e.g. viral, autoimmune, vascular, and others)13,14,15,16. The neurotropism of SARS-CoV-2 may be a contributing factor to the neurological impairments17,18. There is evidence that cognitive impairment can occur in patients with all levels of initial COVID-19 severity19. Several diagnostic tests, addressing visual tasks, were suggested as possible assessment tools for brain health in patients with PCS20,21,22, and other brain related disorders23,24,25. Recent data showed a link between cognitive impairment and visual reaction time (RT) in 2-dimensional (2D) settings of patients with PCS21,22. Findings suggest that cognitive demand in a 3-dimensional (3D) virtual reality seems to differ in comparison to a 2D virtual reality26. Considering the neurotropism of SARS-CoV-2 17,18, eye movement alterations in PCS20, and the correlation between cognitive impairment and RT in PCS21,22, it can be hypothesized that performance in 3-dimensional (3D) vision is reduced in patients with PCS. The aim of this study was to investigate whether RT and correct responses (CR) in a stereoscopic setting can be a distinguishing marker for PCS diagnostics.
Materials and methods
Study population
For a cross-sectional study 179 participants were recruited at the Department of Ophthalmology, Universitätsklinikum Erlangen, Friedrich-Alexander-Universität Erlangen-Nürnberg: 130 patients with PCS and 49 healthy controls. Patients were assigned to the PCS group in accordance with the German Post-Covid guideline27. Exclusion criteria were age below 18 years, pre-existing ocular disorders, pre-existing systemic disorders with ocular involvement, and an uncorrected visual acuity of the worse eye worse than 0.1 (LogMAR). The study has been reviewed and approved by the ethics committee of the Friedrich-Alexander-Universität Erlangen-Nürnberg (295_20 B) and was performed in accordance with the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki. Informed written consent was achieved from each participant prior to enrollment.
Virtual-reality (VR)-oculomotor-test-system (VR-OTS)
The Virtual-Reality-Oculomotor-Test-System (VR-OTS) is a virtual environment constructed to test 3D-vision/stereoscopic performance/depth-perception. VR-OTS is a Medical Device Regulation (MDR)-compliant class 1 medical device. The methodology was described in detail previously28. Shortly, as all four balls were the same size the following issues were addressed: linear perspective was solved by adding a random variation to each ball. Cue overlay of contours was canceled out by avoiding overlaps in the scene. Cue distribution of highlights and shadows was avoided by not casting shadows and using one light source that was far away. Aerial perspective was not rendered in the scene and motion parallax was avoided by attaching the stimulus to the user’s head rotation. Only binocular cues were effective. Monocular depth cues were eliminated, so the participant needed stereopsis to fulfill the task28. The task was presented with VR goggles and was based on binocular cues. The test procedure had to be carried out without the use of glasses or contact lenses. The VR environment showed a stadium-like setting (Fig. 1a): the stimuli (four footballs) were floating in front of the user in a rhomboid arrangement. The stimuli were shown in 9 gaze directions. At the start of the test, one of the balls appeared closer to the user. To create the illusion of one ball appearing closer than the others, a defined horizontal disparity difference was employed relative to the other balls. Three levels of difficulties were established using varying levels of this disparity: 250, 550, 1100 arc-seconds (arcsec, ″). The patients were asked to identify the closest ball of all and consequently to press the corresponding arrow key on a keyboard. The time required to select the correct stereoscopic stimulus is referred to as RT [ms]. CR was defined as percentage of correct responses [decimal]. Each of the three disparities was tested randomly three times at 9 different gaze directions, resulting in 81 inputs per test run. Test time was about 1 min/run (about 5 min/test, including introduction to the test set-up). As none of the participants had any previous experience with the test system, three test runs were done, yet the first two were not scored and used for the patient’s familiarization with the system. The third test run was used for statistical analysis.
Virtual reality environment of the VR-OTS: (a) A sample image of the environment seen by the test person in the VR-OTs is shown. The environment has a stereoscopic impression of depth. The stimuli (four footballs) are floating in front of the user in a rhomboid arrangement in a stadium-like setting; (b) The different stimulus testing positions (gaze directions) are shown schematically. The black cross in the middle of the balls represents the center of this rhomboid arrangement. Nine different directions of gaze were defined in the user’s field of view. Each of the 9 crosses represents a possible test position of the stimuli. The example shown here shows the central position.
VR Headset
In this study, the HTC Vive Pro Eye (HTC Corporation, Taoyuan, Taiwan) was used to create a VR environment. The headset features two 3.5″ Organic-Light-Emitting-Diodes (OLED) displays, each with a resolution of 1440 × 1600 pixels and a refresh rate of 90 Hz. The manufacturer specifies a field of view (FoV) of 110◦. To compensate for variations in the user’s eye relief, the distance between the lenses can be adjusted in a range of 60–73 mm. In addition, the headset is equipped with a built-in Tobii eye tracking system (Tobii AB, Danderyd, Sweden), operating at a sampling rate of 120 Hz, thus achieving an accuracy of 0.5–1.1◦ within a 20◦ FoV. The movements of the headset are tracked in 3D space using HTC’s base stations. Our goal was to use purely stereoscopic visual stimuli that differ in their level of difficulty. For stereoscopic visual stimuli, the level of difficulty can be quantified by disparity differences. The minimum difference in disparities in our stereoscopic visual stimuli is 275″ per pixel28. In order to create different levels of difficulty, only a multiple of the value 275″ was allowed. Three difficulty levels were used (from highest difficulty to lowest difficulty: 275″, 550″, 1100″). By manipulating disparity differences, stimuli were created that would challenge participants’ perceptual abilities and allow for a precise measurement of their performance in virtual 3D vision.
VR environment
The VR environment was designed as follows: the distance of each ball from the center was virtually 25 cm, either vertically or horizontally, and the four balls were defined as Ball-Up, Ball-Down, Ball-Left, and Ball-Right. These definitions were later used to identify, which ball the user was looking at. Nine different directions of gaze were defined within the user’s field of view (Fig. 1b), namely the 8 peripheral (down, lower right, right, upper right, up, upper left, left and lower left) and the central position. It was ensured that the user had to shift his gaze to focus on different stimulus locations. The VR environment followed the user’s head rotation and position so that the relative position of the stimuli to the user was always maintained and couldn’t be changed by turning the head.
Statistical analysis
Variables age, visual acuity, and mean time since COVID-19 were presented before modelling using descriptive measures of center (mean) and variability (standard deviation). A series of generalized Linear Models (GLM) was employed to assess differences between controls and PCS patients, with age and sex included as covariates. This modeling approach, also known as ANCOVA, allowed us to account for the effects of age and sex while assessing the relationships between the primary variables of interest. Specifically, the GLMs were used to examine differences between PCS and control groups across various CR and RT measures, adjusting for these covariates. Separate GLMs were constructed for each independent variable to ensure precise evaluation. Moreover, we ran GLM models on data stratified by Control and PCS groups, with sex as the primary independent variable and age as a covariate. The response variables were CR and RT, with RT calculated as median RTs per each disparity, and gaze direction. The CR were calculated as percentages in decimal of correct inputs in relation to all inputs per each disparity, and gaze direction. Three levels of difficulty, disparities (275″, 550″, 1100″), were randomly tested three times each in nine different viewing directions (8 peripheral and one central position). To ensure robustness, p-values underwent Sidak adjustment. Estimates were calculated with their corresponding confidence interval (95% CIs) to reflect the precision of the results. Age-specific differences describe the estimated effect of age on the outcome, and whether this effect is statistically significant (p-value). Sex-specific differences describe the estimated effect of sex the outcome, and whether this effect is statistically significant (p-value). The two groups (control and PCS) had unequal sample sizes, therefore the LS-Means were calculated in order to provide more robust and accurate comparisons. The LS-Means are descriptive values (means) adjusted for the factors presented in the model (age and sex). Therefore, LS-Means provide a more robust estimate of the effect. Moreover, they are particularly useful in unbalanced designs or when comparing groups with unequal sample sizes. Additionally, Type III Sum of Squares ANOVA were computed to measure the unique contribution of each variable within the models.
To assess the overall significance of the models, F-Tests were conducted for each GLM. The F-Test evaluates whether the model, including the covariates and independent variables, explains a significant portion of the variability in the dependent variables. A significant F-Test indicates that the model as a whole is statistically significant. This was particularly important for confirming that the model appropriately captured the relationship between the primary variables of interest, while adjusting for age and sex. An ANOVA test was performed within each group (PCS and control) for 3D-performance for each disparity and gaze direction. This allowed us to evaluate whether any specific gaze direction was impaired. The dependent variables were CR and median RT, with gaze direction as factor. All the elaborations were done using the SAS version 9.4 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA) and Rstudio ggplot, dplyr and lm packages (Integrated Development for R. RStudio, PBC, Boston, MA).
Results
Demographic data
Of the 179 participants recruited 91 were female and 88 male, with mean age of 35.63 ± 11 years and with a mean uncorrected visual acuity of the worse eye of 0.02 ± 0.04 (LogMAR). The PCS group (130 patients, 66 female, 64 male, mean age 36.91 ± 11 years) had a mean uncorrected visual acuity of the worse eye of 0.02 ± 0.04 (LogMAR). The control group (49 healthy controls, 25 female, 24 male, mean age 32.24 ± 12 years) had a mean uncorrected visual acuity of the worse eye of 0.03 ± 0.04 (LogMAR). Mean time between acute COVID-19 and study participation was 442.49 ± 215 days for the PCS group. Self-reported PCS symptoms were fatigue (including PEM, 92%), reduced concentration (88%), headaches (75%), arrythmia (73%), muscle pain (68%), and POTS (63%).
Overall performance of RT and CR
In terms of overall performance differences in relation to group membership, PCS group showed statistically significant worse (higher) RT than control group in all disparity levels (Table 1), disparity 275″ (p-value = 0.001), 550″ (p-value = 0.001), 1100″ (p-value = 0.003) (Fig. 2D−F). The LS-Means of RT in the PCS group were higher across all difficulty levels (Table 1). It can also be observed that the RT increased with decreasing disparity in both groups (Fig. 3).
In terms of overall performance in relation to group membership, the CR showed statistically significant worse (lower) values of the PCS group than control group in all difficulty levels (Table 1), disparity 275″ (p-value = 0.001), 550″ (p-value = 0.003), 1100″ (p-value = 0.019) (Fig. 2A–C). The LS-Means of CR in the PCS group were lower across all difficulty levels (Table 1). It can also be observed that the CR in both collectives decreases with decreasing disparity (Fig. 3).
Boxplots of CR (A–C) and RT (D–F) in patients with PCS and controls across each disparity (275″, 550″, 1100″). Boxplots of LS-Means of CR [decimal] and RT [ms], adjusted for covariates. An ANCOVA test for group membership was done. Group membership shows statistically significant differences for CR (disparity 275″ p-value < 0.001, 550″ p-value = 0.003, 1100″ p-value = 0.019) and RT (disparity 275″ p-value = 0.001, 550″ p-value = 0.001, 1100″ p-value = 0.003) at all difficulties (disparities). Decreasing CR and increasing RT can be observed for increased difficulty (lower disparity) at all levels for both groups. (CR = correct responses; RT = reaction time; decimal = percentage in decimal; ms = milliseconds; PCS = Post-COVID syndrome; ″=arc-seconds; LS-Means = least square means)
Scatterplots of CR (A–C) and RT (D–F) in patients with PCS and controls considering sex and age for each disparity (275″, 550″, 1100″). Scatterplots of LS-Means of CR [decimal] and RT [ms] are shown. The LS-Means adjust for potential differences between the groups concerning the covariates. Separate plots for males and females display the scored values on the y-axis and the participant’s age on the x-axis. A trend analysis, indicated by the solid line, was conducted. ANCOVA tests were performed for age and sex. Age showed to have statistically significant impact on RT (disparity 275″ p-value < 0.001, 550″ p-value < 0.001, 1100″ p-value < 0.001) at all difficulties (disparities), but for CR just at the hardest difficulty (disparity 275″ p-value = 0.035). Increasing age showed a trend to increase RT and decrease CR. Sex showed statistically significant impact on RT (disparity 275″ p-value = 0.047, 550″ p-value = 0.012, and 1100″ p-value = 0.005) and no statistically significant impact on CR for all disparities. Men exhibit lower RT compared to women. The PCS group’s trendline generally lies below that of the control group, indicating lower CR values across all age levels. Notable exceptions include CR at 275″ for males older than 60 years and RT at 550″ for males older than 60 years, as well as at 1100″ for males older than 55 years. (CR = correct responses; RT = reaction time; decimal = percentage in decimal; ms = milliseconds; PCS = Post-COVID syndrome; ″=arc-seconds; LS-Means = least square means)
Sub-analysis of RT and CR according to gaze directions
A sub-analysis was conducted to compare RT and CR between PCS and control group across various individual gaze directions, in which the stimuli were presented (Tables 2 and 3).
In terms of performance in individual gaze direction the PCS group consistently showed increased RT across all disparities and gaze directions, with many of these differences reaching statistical significance (Table 2). At disparity 275″, five out of nine gaze directions showed a statistically significant difference (Table 2). At disparity 550″ and 1100″ all nine gaze directions showed a statistically significant difference (Table 2).
Similarly, for CR, patients with PCS consistently demonstrated lower CR across all disparities and gaze directions, with numerous differences achieving to be statistically significant (Table 3). At disparity 275″ six out of nine gaze directions, at disparity 550″ four out of nine gaze directions, and at disparity 1100″ three out of nine showed a statistically significant difference (Table 3).
No statistically significant differences (p-values > 0.5, provided by separate ANOVAs for PCS and control) were found between gaze directions for RT and CR across different disparities.
Analysis of the covariates (age, sex) on RT and CR
We analyzed the effects of the covariates age and sex on RT and CR in the total cohort (Table 1). Statistically significant effects of age on RT were observed across all disparities, 275” (p-value < 0.001), 550” (p-value < 0.001), and 1100” (p-value < 0.001), indicating an increasing trend in RT with age (refer to Fig. 3D-F). Men generally exhibited lower RTs than women, with significant differences at disparities 275” (p-value = 0.047), 550” (p-value = 0.012), and 1100” (p-value = 0.005). Regarding CR, age showed a statistically significant impact only at the disparity of 275” (p-value = 0.035), with a negative trend in CR values as age increased (Fig. 3A). No significant age effects on CR were observed at disparities of 550” (p-value = 0.459) and 1100” (p-value = 0.893). Additionally, no statistically significant differences were found for the effect of sex on CR across all disparities (275” p-value = 0.089, 550” p-value = 0.067, and 1100” p-value = 0.688).
In addition, we performed a sub-analysis of the effect of sex on the variables (RT and CR) for patients with PCS and controls, respectively (Table 4). RT of female patients with PCS was significantly increased across all disparities (275” p-value = 0.03, 550” p-value = 0.015, and 1100” p-value = 0.009) compared to male patients with PCS. Contrary, RT was not statistically significantly different between the sexes in controls (disparity 275” p-value = 0.991, 550” p-value = 0.548, and 1100” p-value = 0.29). Regarding CR, sex showed no statistically significant difference for RT or CR in the PCS or control group, except for CR at disparity 275” of PCS (p-value = 0.044, Table 4).
Discussion
With the emergence of the SARS-CoV-2 pandemic, a symptom complex called PCS is increasingly coming into focus alongside various acute and extensive sequelae. A broad variety of symptoms are reported, which appear to affect a wide range of organ systems7,8,9,10,11,12, including partly severely debilitating neurological and cognitive symptoms19,29,30. PCS can result in a massive decrease of patients’ quality of life: patients can feel moderate up to severe impairment performing household tasks or general work31, some aren’t able to work6,32 or even need intense care in all aspects of life33,34. Thus, PCS can have a serious impact on people’s autonomy, mental health, their social life, and their ability to go back to work35. This results in a significant economic impact for the patients themselves and society, respectively6,35. Right now, an objective diagnosis or objectively standardized diagnostic criteria are not yet available and are needed for regular clinical all-day life36. SARS-CoV-2 is known to show a neurotropism17,18. Considering eye movement alterations in patients with PCS20 and a correlation between cognitive impairment and RT in patients with PCS in a 2D-environment21,22, it can be hypothesized that performance in 3-dimensional (3D) vision is reduced in patients with PCS. This study investigated the RT during virtual-3D-environment testing in patients with PCS and found a worsening compared to controls. Visual tasks demanding stereoscopic capability were performed using a virtual reality setting. The PCS patients performed significantly worse in all difficulties regarding CR and RT. Our sub-analysis revealed no significant discrepancy in any gaze direction to be particularly impaired among the observed difficulties, therefore it can be argued that the worse performance of PCS seems to be homogeneously distributed across all gaze directions. Looking at the different performance per level of difficulty for both groups poorer performance was found in terms of CR and RT with increasing difficulty achieved through decreasing disparities.
Different testing approaches for measuring visuals tasks in patients after COVID-19 were described20,21,22,28. A video-oculography study design showed eye movement alterations after symptomatic COVID-19 in comparison to controls20. The combination of a questionnaire of self-reported Long-COVID symptoms and a self-administered visual detection task yielded that the parameters ‘bad cognition’, ‘poor physical condition’, ‘learning difficulties’, and ‘visual impairment’ were the best predictors for a prolonged RT in their 2D-visual tasks21. Yet, these data were collected without comparison to a control group. An oculomotor, vestibular, RT test system with eye tracking showed altered optokinetic nystagmus and saccades in patients after COVID-19 compared to normative data22.
All these methods used a 2D-environement, contrary to the VR-OTS method, using a virtual-3D-environment. The VR-OTS method has already been used in a proof-of-concept study28. The method is based on previous works from Paulus et al.37, and Schoemann et al.38. A stereoscopic task uses different disparities between the test objects. The task was presented with VR goggles and was based solely on binocular cues. For stereoscopic visual stimuli, the level of difficulty was quantified by disparity differences. The minimum difference in disparities in our stereoscopic visual stimuli were 275″ per pixel28. By manipulating disparity differences, stimuli that would challenge participants’ perceptual abilities were created.
Virtual 3D testing was described in several brain associated diseases like Parkinson’s disease23,24, Alzheimer’s disease23, mild traumatic brain injuries25, and depression30 previously. Ba et al.24 used a 3D active shutter system and found patients with Parkinson’s disease to have worse stereopsis, longer visual response times, and eye movement alterations, which correlated positively with motor function and negatively with cognitive status. Kara et al.25 used the VR-OTS in a similar manner than this study used, in patients with concussion. They showed patients with mild and moderate traumatic brain injury to have impaired stereopsis. The response time and error rate differences were statistically significant. Paulus et al.37 were using a 3D-TV with a static and dynamic stereo test, one consisting of 4 soccer balls and found no significantly better performance of soccer players against no soccer player in their stereopsis tests, even though they performed better in a monocular simple choice test regarding RT. They also found a significant increase in RT with decreasing disparity differences. Effects on stereopsis were also investigated in non-disease related contexts such as sports37 and aging39.
The results of the present study confirm the previous data of a proof-of-concept study28. Both variables, RT and CR, were significantly impaired in patients with PCS compared to controls (Table 1). As the parameter CR represents the correct responses, RT and CR do not affect each other. Both parameters showed a similar impairment in any gaze direction. Other studies also showed a worse performance in visual tasks in PCS collectives21,22,40. Santoyo-Mora et al. showed the need of a higher visual sensitivity and processing speed after severe COVID-19 40. Significant differences in RT for choosing the correct stereoscopic stimulus were found ranging from 147 ms, 167 ms to 183 ms for RT from easiest to hardest difficulty.
For the effect of age on CR the PCS group’s trendline consistently remained in a lower position compared to the controls, indicating an effect on all levels of age (Fig. 3). For the effect of age on RT statistically significant results were found. A trend that RT is increasing with an increased age was observed. This could be attributed to the described effect of aging on processing speed41,42,43. Studies suggested a link between visual impairment and cognitive decline in middle ages and older people44,45,46. Crivelli et al.10 also showed a rise in cognitive dysfunction after COVID-19 with increasing age.
In our data set male and female participants performed significantly differently regarding RT (Table 1). The RT of female PCS patients was increased compared to male patients at all levels of difficulty, yet not in controls (Table 4). Regarding the sex effect for CR, no sex effect was observed in either PCS or controls, except for PCS at disparity 275″ (Table 4). These findings could support the trend, in which women exhibited cognitive impairment more often than male after COVID-19 36.
The observed impaired RT and CR in patients with PCS could be attributed to different factors: neuronal perception time, motoric reaction and maximum speed (including the muscles of the eye), neuronal recording time and processing time could contribute singularly or combined to a decreasing stereoscopic performance. The findings of the present study might be associated with prolonged saccade latencies, impaired saccade accuracy, increased corrective movements and fixations. More fixations lead to longer processing times as the brain has to integrate more visual data. In addition, impaired smooth pursuit movements might lead to fragmented and less efficient visual tracking. Further studies could address the singular factors for an enhanced understanding of the pathomechanistic alterations.
The study is not without limitations. Visual acuity was required to not be worse than 0.1 (logMAR) (without correction), as the test set-up did not allow wearing of glasses during the VR-OTS test. In addition, the study design was cross-sectional. It would be of interest, if long-term data could confirm an improvement or worsening of the PCS symptoms over a period of time. As the VR-OTS test duration is even short (about 5 min) and easy to handle, the VR-OTS seems to be suitable for patients with fatigue or PEM.
Conclusion
The data of the present study showed that a stereoscopic impairment of RT and CR might be a novel diagnostic marker in PCS diagnostics.
Data availability
The datasets are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.
References
Katella, K. Our Pandemic Year—A COVID-19 Timeline > News > Yale Medicine, https://www.yalemedicine.org/news/covid-timeline (2021).
WHO & Coronavirus (COVID-19) Dashboard | WHO Coronavirus (COVID-19) Dashboard With Vaccination Data, https://covid19.who.int/?adgroupsurvey=%7Badgroupsurvey%7D&gclid=Cj0KCQiAw8OeBhCeARIsAGxWtUxR9wuVfKmK8awaDLSRrSm65bKiEJOcvWX34XpP2LX4eD_sR9ZfUqAaArNPEALw_wcB>, (2023).
Soriano, J. B., Murthy, S., Marshall, J. C., Relan, P. & Diaz, J. V. In The Lancet Infectious Diseases, 22 e102-e107Elsevier Ltd, (2022).
A clinical case definition of post COVID-19 condition by a Delphi consensus, 6 October < (2021). https://www.who.int/publications/i/item/WHO-2019-nCoV-Post_COVID-19_condition-Clinical_case_definition-2021.1 (2021).
Ballering, A. V., van Zon, S. K. R., Hartman, Rosmalen, J. G. & T. C. & M. Persistence of somatic symptoms after COVID-19 in the Netherlands: an observational cohort study. Lancet (London England). 400, 452–461. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(22)01214-4 (2022).
Nittas, V. et al. Frontiers Media S.A.,. in Public Health Reviews, 43 (2022).
Chen, C. et al. Global prevalence of Post-coronavirus Disease 2019 (COVID-19) Condition or Long COVID: a Meta-analysis and systematic review. J. Infect. Dis.226, 1593–1607. https://doi.org/10.1093/infdis/jiac136 (2022).
Alkodaymi, M. S. et al. Clin. Microbiol. Infect.28 657–666, Elsevier B.V., (2022).
Badenoch, J. B. et al. In Brain Communications 4 (Oxford University Press, 2022).
Crivelli, L. et al. John Wiley and Sons Inc,. in Alzheimer’s Dementia, 18 1047–1066 (2022).
Han, Q., Zheng, B., Daines, L. & Sheikh, A. in Pathogens, 11MDPI, (2022).
Mazza, M. G., Palladini, M., Poletti, S. & Benedetti, F. CNS Drugs36 681–702 (Adis, (2022).
Castanares-Zapatero, D. et al. Taylor and Francis Ltd.,. In Ann. Med., 54 1473–1487 (2022).
Pierce, J. D., Shen, Q., Cintron, S. A. & Hiebert, J. B. Post-COVID-19 syndrome. Nurs. Res.71, 164–174. https://doi.org/10.1097/NNR.0000000000000565 (2022).
Mehandru, S. & Merad, M. in Nature Immunology, 23 194–202Nature Research, (2022).
Batiha, G. E. S., Al-kuraishy, H. M., Al-Gareeb, A. I. & Welson, N. N. in Virology Journal Vol. 19BioMed Central Ltd, (2022).
Mendonça Filho, V. C. M. et al. COVID-19 in the nervous system: physiopathology and neurological manifestations. Arquivos de neuro-psiquiatria 81, 756–763 https://doi.org/10.1055/S-0043-1769123 (2023).
Wu, Y. et al. Nervous system involvement after infection with COVID-19 and other coronaviruses. Brain. Behav. Immun.87, 18–18. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.BBI.2020.03.031 (2020).
Schilling, C., Meyer-Lindenberg, A. & Schweiger J. I. in Nervenarzt, 93 779–787Springer Medizin, (2022).
García Cena, C. E. et al. Eye Movement alterations in Post-COVID-19 Condition: a proof-of-Concept Study. Sens. (Basel Switzerland) 22. https://doi.org/10.3390/S22041481 (2022).
Maiorana, N. V. et al. Relationship between Reaction Times and Post-COVID-19 symptoms assessed by a web-based visual detection Task. Healthc. (Basel Switzerland). 11https://doi.org/10.3390/HEALTHCARE11030284 (2023).
Kelly, K. M. et al. Oculomotor, vestibular, reaction time, and cognitive tests as objective measures of neural deficits in patients post COVID-19 infection. Front. Neurol. 13. https://doi.org/10.3389/FNEUR.2022.919596 (2022).
Lee, C. N., Ko, D., Suh, Y. W. & Park, K. W. Cognitive functions and stereopsis in patients with Parkinson’s disease and Alzheimer’s disease using 3-dimensional television: a case controlled trial. PloS One. 10. https://doi.org/10.1371/JOURNAL.PONE.0123229 (2015).
Ba, F. et al. Stereopsis and Eye Movement Abnormalities in Parkinson’s Disease and Their Clinical Implications. Front. Aging Neurosci.14https://doi.org/10.3389/FNAGI.2022.783773 (2022).
Kara, D. D., Ring, M., Hennig, F. F. & Michelson, G. Effects of mild traumatic brain Injury on Stereopsis detected by a virtual reality system: attempt to develop a screening test. J. Med. Biol. Eng.40, 639–647. https://doi.org/10.1007/S40846-020-00542-7/FIGURES/6 (2020).
Dan, A. & Reiner, M. EEG-based cognitive load of processing events in 3D virtual worlds is lower than processing events in 2D displays. Int. J. Psychophysiology: Official J. Int. Organ. Psychophysiol.122, 75–84. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.IJPSYCHO.2016.08.013 (2017).
Koczulla, A. R. et al. [German S1 Guideline Long-/Post-COVID]. Pneumologie. 76, 855–907. https://doi.org/10.1055/a-1946-3230 (2022).
Mehringer, W. et al. Virtual reality for assessing stereopsis performance and eye characteristics in Post-COVID. Sci. Rep.13, 13167–13167. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-40263-w (2023).
Vannorsdall, T. D. et al. Cognitive dysfunction, Psychiatric Distress, and functional decline after COVID-19. J. Acad. Consultation-Liaison Psychiatry. 63, 133–143. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaclp.2021.10.006 (2022).
Zawilska, J. B. & Kuczyńska, K. in Journal of Psychiatric Research Vol. 156 349–360Elsevier Ltd, (2022).
Zhao, Y. et al. Follow-up study of the pulmonary function and related physiological characteristics of COVID-19 survivors three months after recovery. EClinicalMedicine. 25https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eclinm.2020.100463 (2020).
Delgado-Alonso, C. et al. Fatigue and cognitive dysfunction are Associated with Occupational Status in Post-COVID Syndrome. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 19https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph192013368 (2022).
Malik, P. et al. Post-acute COVID-19 syndrome (PCS) and health-related quality of life (HRQoL)—A systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Med. Virol.94, 253–262. https://doi.org/10.1002/jmv.27309 (2022).
Patel, U. K. et al. Long-term neurological sequelae among severe COVID-19 patients: a systematic review and Meta-analysis. Cureus. 14https://doi.org/10.7759/cureus.29694 (2022).
Rajan, S. et al. In in the wake of the Pandemic: Preparing for Long COVID (European Observatory on Health Systems and Policies, 2021).
Ceban, F. et al. In Brain, Behavior, and ImmunityVol. 10193–135 (Academic Press Inc., 2022).
Paulus, J. et al. Extended stereopsis evaluation of professional and amateur soccer players and subjects without soccer background. Front. Psychol.5, 1186–1186. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01186 (2014).
Schoemann, M. D., Lochmann, M., Paulus, J. & Michelson, G. Repetitive dynamic stereo test improved processing time in young athletes. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci.35, 413–421. https://doi.org/10.3233/RNN-170729 (2017).
Norman, J. F. et al. Stereopsis and aging. Vision. Res.48, 2456–2465. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.VISRES.2008.08.008 (2008).
Santoyo-Mora, M. et al. COVID-19 Long-Term Effects: Is There an Impact on the Simple Reaction Time and Alternative-Forced Choice on Recovered Patients? Brain sciences12, 1258. https://doi.org/10.3390/BRAINSCI12091258 (2022).
Fjell, A. M. & Walhovd, K. B. Structural brain changes in aging: courses, causes and cognitive consequences. Rev. Neurosci.21, 187–221. https://doi.org/10.1515/REVNEURO.2010.21.3.187 (2010).
Kail, R. & Salthouse, T. A. Processing speed as a mental capacity. Acta. Psychol.86, 199–225. https://doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(94)90003-5 (1994).
Ebaid, D., Crewther, S. G., MacCalman, K., Brown, A. & Crewther, D. P. Cognitive Processing speed across the Lifespan: beyond the influence of Motor Speed. Front. Aging Neurosci.9https://doi.org/10.3389/FNAGI.2017.00062 (2017).
Zhao, X. et al. Associations of sensory impairment and cognitive function in middle-aged and older Chinese population: the China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study. J. Global Health. 11https://doi.org/10.7189/JOGH.11.08008 (2021).
Maharani, A., Dawes, P., Nazroo, J., Tampubolon, G. & Pendleton, N. Visual and hearing impairments are associated with cognitive decline in older people. Age Ageing. 47, 575–581. https://doi.org/10.1093/AGEING/AFY061 (2018).
Maharani, A. et al. Associations between Self-reported sensory impairment and risk of Cognitive decline and impairment in the Health and Retirement Study Cohort. J. Gerontol. B. 75, 1230–1242. https://doi.org/10.1093/GERONB/GBZ043 (2020).
Acknowledgements
The present work was performed in (partial) fulfillment of the requirements for obtaining the degree “Dr. med.”/”Dr. med. dent.”.Performed at Friedrich-Alexander-Universität Erlangen-Nürnberg (FAU).
Funding
The present study was funded by the (1) Bavarian Health and Food Safety Authority (LGL; 2490-PC-2021-V14: Discover – Establishment and evaluation of clinical algorithms for objective diagnosis of subtypes of Long-COVID as an essential basis for effective patient care), the (2) Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG; GRK 2504/1: Novel antiviral approaches from small molecules to immune intervention), and the (3) Federal Ministry of Education and Research (BMBF; iIMMUNE_ACS; 01EO2105: iIMMUNE_ACS - Förderung von Advanced Clinician Scientists im Bereich Immunmedizin).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
Conceptualization: BH, MG, CM, GM; Data curation: MG, ML; Formal analysis: ML; Funding acquisition: BH, CM, GM; Investigation: MG, FS, JZ; Methodology: WM, GM; Project administration: BH, MG, FS, JZ; Software: WM, GM; Supervision: BH, GM; Visualization: MG, ML, BH; Writing – original draft: MG, BH, ML, WM; Writing – review and editing: BH, MG, GM, ML, AS, RH, FS, JZ, CM, WM, MGa. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
G.M. is owner of Talkingeyes and More GmbH, which developed VR-OTS. The remaining authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher’s note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License, which permits any non-commercial use, sharing, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if you modified the licensed material. You do not have permission under this licence to share adapted material derived from this article or parts of it. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Güttes, M., Lucio, M., Skornia, A. et al. A case-control study of reaction time deficits in a 3D virtual reality in patients with Post-COVID syndrome. Sci Rep 14, 27204 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-76827-7
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-76827-7