Abstract
Studies on animal temperaments (consistent differences in behaviors across contexts) and behavioral syndromes (suites of correlated behaviors across contexts) have surged in recent decades. Accordingly, behavioral ecologists have gained greater appreciation for their evolutionary role and significance. Yet, despite their importance as potential evolutionary drivers, research focused on temperament and syndromes in shaping hybridization events is vastly understudied. Case studies have shown that hybridization has multiple effects on these phenomena, such as eliminating syndromes present in parental lineages and generating novel syndromes within hybrids. Here, we assessed temperament and syndromes in a naturally occurring rattlesnake hybrid zone (Crotalus scutulatus × viridis). We used laboratory behavioral assays to quantify defensive and explorative behaviors, and tested whether these traits were correlated with spatial and hunting behaviors of free-ranging individuals. C. viridis was more significantly more prone to rattle than C. scutulatus during handling tests. Similarly, hybrid individuals that had a greater proportion of their genome derived from C. viridis were also more prone to rattle. Parental and hybrid snakes exhibited varying syndromes in defensiveness and exploratory behaviors, yet further research is necessary to determine whether they impact hybrid fitness by creating mismatches between temperaments and predation pressures under natural conditions.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
Behavior has long been recognized as both a highly variable and evolutionarily labile aspect of an organism’s phenotype, and an increasing number of studies in recent decades document consistent patterns of behaviors (i.e., temperament) expressed by individuals across different contexts1,2. Temperament (also referred to in the literature as “animal personality”) is typically measured on binary (e.g., bold vs. shy, explorative vs. non-explorative, aggressive vs. submissive) or continuous scales. For example, individuals that are on the bolder side of the bold/shy continuum are predicted to prioritize high risk/high reward behaviors consistently across different contexts (e.g., time, age, reproductive status, foraging, and social interactions). Temperaments appear to be moderately heritable and thus shaped by both environmental and genetic variation3. Behavioral ecologists also make a distinction between temperament and behavioral syndromes, which are defined by the correlation between two or more behavioral temperaments expressed by an individual across time or context4,5.
Although behavioral variation associated with temperament and behavioral syndromes (henceforth termed syndromes) has been studied widely across taxa and in a number of ecological and evolutionary contexts1,3,6,7,8,9,10,11, our understanding of these phenomena in the context of interspecific hybridization is understudied. In hybrid populations, for example, genetic and phenotypic variability is often higher between individuals owing to interspecific gene flow from different parental lineages12. This type of admixture could theoretically lead to the breakdown of suites of correlated traits—which may lead to hybrid inferiority when co-adapted traits optimize fitness13. Accordingly, syndromes might collapse across hybrid zones and, in turn, represent an extrinsic barrier to further hybridization, assuming their collapse leads to non-optimal expression of behaviors in critical environmental contexts14. Conversely, selection can also have a role in strengthening syndromes in hybrids15, but there is mixed support for this outcome.
Hybrids between benthic and planktivorous morphs of Arctic Charr (Salvelinus alpinus) have been shown to express repeatability in boldness and sociability, but to a lesser degree than the parental individuals. In this case, no evidence for the relaxation of behavioral syndromes in hybrids was found14. However, other studies have found that syndromes in hybrids tend to be weaker than in parentals. Parental salmon morphs (Salmo salar) expressed distinct syndromes associated with boldness, aggression, and exploration, whereas hybrids only displayed a syndrome between boldness and response to a novel object16. Similarly, hybrids of two pufferfish species (Takifugu rubripes and T. niphobles) had similar, but statistically weaker, syndromes than the parentals in a variety of temperament traits, including boldness and feeding responses17. Finally, hybrids can express unique and/or stronger syndromes than parentals. Hybrid swordtails (Xiphophorus spp.) showed a significant syndrome between boldness and defensiveness that was absent in the parental populations15. This situation is akin to the expression of transgressive or novel traits seen in many hybrid zones18,19,20, which can lead to adaptive evolution through transgressive segregation in the hybrids21. Though research is limited, temperament can act as a post-zygotic barrier if certain temperaments that are over- or under-expressed in hybrids are favored by natural selection in the habitat where hybridization occurs. Interestingly, a recent review specifically highlighted the potential for temperament to drive reproductive isolation22.
Sexual selection can favor either assortative or disassortative mating based on individual temperaments. In turn, this can shape pre-zygotic isolating mechanisms between species. For example, temperaments can shape spatial behaviors and habitat selection in ways that increase or decrease isolation between lineages, depending on context22. Additionally, a large number of studies have demonstrated how variation related to temperaments and syndromes shapes traits related to reproductive isolation, including variation in general activity levels23,24,25,26, exploration patterns24,25,27,28, dispersal29,30, foraging activity7,26,31, spatial behaviors32, anti-predator behaviors25,33,34,35,36, and reproductive success7,8,27,37,38.
Unquestionably, the literature on animal temperament is taxonomically uneven, with relatively few studies of non-avian reptiles1,39. Only two previous studies have examined temperament in pitviper snakes (Viperidae: Crotalinae). These studies found that rattlesnakes (Southern Pacific Rattlesnakes, C. oreganus, and Western Diamond-backed Rattlesnakes, C. atrox, respectively) exhibited quantifiable behavioral traits that were consistent within individuals while also showing syndromes in C. atrox with respect to movement activity, boldness, and sociability40,41 Because signatures of interspecific hybridization have been found repeatedly in C. scutulatus42, C. viridis and C. oreganus43,44 clades, these are ideal taxa in which to study the role of temperament and syndromes in shaping hybridization dynamics.
Here, we studied temperament and syndromes in rattlesnakes from a narrow band (12 km) of transitional/mosaic habitat region between the Chihuahuan and Sonoran deserts in southwestern New Mexico where Mojave Rattlesnakes (Crotalus scutulatus) and Prairie Rattlesnakes (C. viridis) hybridize. Specifically, we tested a large cohort of parental and hybrid individuals to quantify behavioral traits related to defensiveness and exploration. Furthermore, we assessed potential syndromes between temperament traits expressed in laboratory assays and both spatial behaviors45and hunting behaviors46 in free-ranging snakes to assess whether there are correlations between behaviors expressed in a captive context and the more ecologically relevant (but logistically challenging to acquire) field behaviors. Based on the potential for hybrids to exhibit increased variation in temperaments, we hypothesized that hybrids would be transgressive or intermediate, depending on the relative expression of temperament traits in parental species. We also hypothesized that parental populations would exhibit syndromes across defensive, exploratory, and spatial/hunting contexts, but that these syndromes would be weakly expressed (or absent) in hybrids.
Results
Behavioral assays
We conducted behavioral assays on 185 rattlesnakes: 41 Mojave Rattlesnakes (Crotalus scutulatus; adults = 36; juveniles = 5; male = 30; non-pregnant females = 11), 59 Prairie Rattlesnakes (C. viridis; adults = 43; juveniles = 16; male = 43; non-pregnant females = 16), and 85 hybrid rattlesnakes (C. scutulatus × viridis; adults = 56; juveniles = 29; male = 53; non-pregnant females = 32). Pregnant females were not included in this study due to the lack of sample size (C. scutulatus = 1, C. viridis = 3, and C. scutulatus × viridis = 6). The most informative models for explaining variation in rattle behavior were those containing genetic group + age and genetic group * age. Therefore, we report the results from the model containing genetic group + age as the two predictor variables. We found both genetic group (X2 = 20.8, df = 2, p < 0.001) and age (X2 = 9.38, df = 1, p = 0.002) were significant predictors of rattling behavior. Crotalus scutulatus rattled less frequently (26.8% of individuals rattled defensively) than C. viridis (59.3%; post-hoc Tukey: z-ratio = −3.52, p = 0.001), but were not significantly different than the hybrids (25.6%; post-hoc Tukey: z-ratio = −0.247, p = 0.967). Hybrid subjects also rattled significantly less frequently than C. viridis (post-hoc Tukey: z-ratio = 3.98, p < 0.001; Fig. 1). Overall, adult snakes of all categories were more likely to rattle (42.2% rattled) than juveniles (20% rattled; Fig. 2).
Bar graph of the proportions of snakes that rattled during the handling assay. Propensity to rattle was significantly different across groups (X2 = 20.8; df = 2; p < 0.001). More Crotalus viridis rattled than either C. scutulatus or C. scutulatus × viridis (post-hoc Tukey: z-ratio = −3.52, 3.98; p = 0.001, < 0.001; respectively), whereas there was no difference between C. scutulatus and C. scutulatus × viridis (post-hoc Tukey: z-ratio = −0.247, p = 0.967). Letters above the bars indicate statistically significant groupings of the genetic groups. Sample sizes: C. scutulatus = 41, C. viridis = 59, C. scutulatus × viridis = 85.
During exploratory assays, snakes spent an average of 8.17 min in hideboxes (13.6% of the total time), but this was highly variable (Table 1). For time spent in hidebox, the model with genetic group as the sole fixed factor was the most informative for analysis. However, the relationship between group and time in hidebox was not significant (X2 = 0.231; df = 2, 181; p = 0.891). Groups also did not differ in the variability of this behavior (F = 0.392; df = 2, 181; p = 0.677).
For the number of quadrant transitions, the most informative model contained only genetic group as the predictor variable and no significance was found between number of quadrant transitions and genetic group (X2 = 2.05, df = 2,181, p = 0.360). Snakes performed an average of six quadrant transitions during the 60 min exploratory assay, but were equally variable within and between groups (F = 0.773; df = 2, 181; p = 0.463; Table 1).
For time spent motionless during open field tests, the most informative models contained genetic group and genetic group + age as the fixed factors, and so we report the results of the model with only genetic group. Again, we found no difference between the groups in the amount of time that they spent motionless (X2 = 0.692; df = 2, 181; p = 0.708). Snakes spent an average of 34.7 min (57.9%) motionless outside of a hidebox. The variability of this trait was also high, but not significantly different between the groups (F = 0.867; df = 2, 181; p = 0.422; Table 1).
For individuals tested in the threat assay (C. scutulatus = 32, C. viridis = 47, C. scutulatus × viridis = 55), the model containing genetic group was the most informative. Overall, only 24.6% of the snakes struck defensively and we found no relationship between the number of snakes that struck during the assay and their genetic group (X2 = 3.26, df = 2, p = 0.196; Figure S1).
Behavior and hybrid index
For our analysis of hybrid index (HI—proportion of the genome derived from C. viridis) and rattling behavior, the most informative models contained HI and HI + age. Hence, we report the results of the model containing HI as the only fixed factor. Individuals with higher HIs (i.e., more C. viridis-like) were significantly more likely to rattle during the handling assay (X2 = 5.45, df = 1, p = 0.020; Fig. 3).
The most informative models for time in a hidebox contained HI, HI + age, and HI * age, time spent motionless contained HI, HI + age, HI * age, and HI * age + sex, and number of quadrant transitions contained HI * age, HI * age + sex as the predictor variables. Therefore, we report the results of the model containing HI as the only predictor variable for the time a hybrid snake spent in a hidebox and motionless and the results of the model containing HI * age for the number of quadrant transactions snakes performed. None of the exploratory behaviors had a significant relationship with the hybrid index (time spent in a hidebox [X2 = 0.417, df = 1, p = 0.519], number of quadrant transitions [X2 = 0.138, df = 1, p = 0.710], and time spent motionless [X2 = 0.585, df = 1, p = 0.445]). We initially found that the interaction between age and HI of the snakes significantly affected the number of quadrant transitions (X2 = 7.78, df = 1, p = 0.005), but visualization of this relationship resulted in identification of two statistical outliers in the juvenile sample (using the Minimum Covariance Determinant method since the data was heavily skewed); when reanalyzing the model without these two outliers the number of quadrant transitions was not significantly related to the age of hybrid snakes (X2 = 3.513, df = 1, p = 0.061) and the interaction between age and HI of the snakes was not significant (X2 = 3.232, df = 1, p = 0.072). A plot of this relationship is presented in Figure S.2. The most informative model for striking during the threat assay contained only HI. We did not find a significant relationship between hybrid index and striking (X2 = 0.190, df = 1, p = 0.663).
Behavioral syndromes: defensiveness vs. exploration
We found that the number of behavioral syndromes expressed among individuals differed between groups. Adult C. scutulatus displayed behavioral syndromes between defensive striking (i.e., striking during the threat assay) and time spent motionless (X2 = 3.99, df = 1, p = 0.046, Table 2) and quadrant transitions (X2 = 7.85, df = 1, p = 0.005, Table 2). Crotalus scutulatus that spent more time motionless were more likely to strike during the threat assay, and C. scutulatus that performed more quadrant transitions were also less likely to strike during the threat assay. Crotalus viridis exhibited significant relationships between time in a hidebox and defensive striking (X2 = 6.21, df = 1, p = 0.013, Table 2), time spent motionless and defensive striking (X2 = 6.71, df = 1, p = 0.010, Table 2), and quadrant transitions and defensive striking (X2 = 5.80, df = 1, p = 0.016, Table 2). Adult C. viridis who spent more time within a hidebox or motionless were less likely to strike during the threat assay. However, like C. scutulatus, C. viridis that performed more quadrant transitions were also less likely to strike during the threat assay. We found two behavioral syndromes in C. scutulatus × viridis: adult hybrids that spent more time motionless or performed more quadrant transitions, were less likely to rattle during the handling assay (X2 = 6.22, 9.28; df = 1, 1; p = 0.013, 0.002; Table 2). We did not find any evidence of behavioral syndromes in juvenile subjects of C. viridis or C. scutulatus × viridis. Owing to small sample size (n = 5), we were not able to analyze behavioral syndromes of juvenile C. scutulatus.
Behavioral syndromes: exploration vs. field behaviors
We found no evidence for behavioral syndromes within any of the groups between spatial or hunting behaviors measured in the field and exploratory behaviors assayed in the laboratory (Tables 3 and 4). After combining data from all groups, we found a moderately strong syndrome (r = − 0.386, p = 0.032) between spatial behavior (number of days per movement) and exploratory behavior, wherein snakes that moved more often in the field also tended to transition between quadrants more often during the exploratory assay (Fig. 4).
Scatter plot showing the negative correlation (behavioral syndrome) between the number of days per movement for individuals when they were free-ranging and the number of quadrant transitions those individuals performed during the exploratory assay. (Spearman correlation, Holm’s adjustment: r = 0.−386, n = 43, p = 0.032).
Discussion
Our general findings from the behavior assays indicated that Prairie Rattlesnakes (Crotalus viridis), Mojave Rattlesnakes (C. scutulatus), and their hybrids (C. scutulatus × viridis) showed broadly similar behavior to each other, except in a few key traits. Prairie Rattlesnakes were more likely to rattle defensively compared to Mojave Rattlesnakes. Furthermore, hybrid individuals with a larger proportion of their genome derived from C. viridis also were more likely to rattle, indicating a potential genetic basis for this eponymous defensive trait. Across all three groups, snakes were generally hesitant to strike at threats and exhibited similar levels of exploration within a novel environment. Although C. viridis displayed the most correlated trait pairs, all three groups showed evidence of some syndromes between defensiveness and exploration/activity, although particulars of the syndromes differed across groups.
Because behavioral temperament can have a number of direct and indirect effects on fitness47,48,49, it is important to consider details of how these traits are expressed and measured in different contexts. The only behavioral trait we measured that differed significantly between groups was rattling behavior during the handling assay, an expression of defensiveness50. Defensive rattling, like most behaviors, is undoubtedly shaped by individual experience, ecological context, and genetic variability. The C. viridis we used in this study may have had more exposure to predators, as previous work found a higher number of predator encounters in the habitat occupied by C. viridis compared to areas occupied by C. scutulatusand hybrid individuals51. Thus, an increased propensity to rattle may be, in part, a product of stronger selection against individuals that do not react defensively toward potential predators or enemies36,52,53,54. However, an increase in the propensity to rattle was also significantly related to the proportion of the genome derived from C. viridis (HI) within hybrid individuals. Additionally, hybrid individuals with more prairie-like genomes were not spatially clustered, so it is unclear what environmental pressures would lead to this increased propensity to rattle.
Antipredator behaviors in a number of taxa have been shown to be heritable, with examples reported in insects55,56, mammals57,58, fish59,60, cephalopods33, birds61,62, and lizards63, but no studies exist for other non-avian reptiles. To our knowledge, the correlation we found between HI and rattling is the first empirical evidence pointing toward the potential for a genetic basis for the expression of an antipredator display in rattlesnakes. Future analyses incorporating genome-wide association studies and transcriptomic approaches could potentially identify genes or regions of the genome underpinning this unique aposematic behavior.
A large body of literature has shown that behavioral temperaments are often correlated across contexts5, and these syndromes can impact fitness either synergistically or antagonistically depending on context. Common syndromes seen across taxa include a positive correlation between boldness and general activity7,24,25,64,65, boldness and explorativeness24,25,64,66,67,68,69, and boldness and foraging34,53,70. Perhaps because it is associated with locating resources in the environment, the propensity to explore novel environments is often correlated with a number of other traits besides boldness, including general activity64,71,72, aggressiveness68, sociability73, and docility74,75. Our study adds to this body of work by showing evidence for a syndrome between the exploratory behaviors of rattlesnakes and how often they move within the landscape.
Since hybrid individuals are a product of two distinct parental genotypes, we hypothesized that syndromes would be generally weaker in hybrids16,17, but our results did not necessarily show that pattern. Although evidence for syndromes between defensiveness and exploration/activity was found in all groups, the particular behaviors that correlated across these contexts varied among the groups, and were not consistent among contexts within groups. Crotalus viridis, individuals that struck defensively exhibited higher levels of activity in novel environments by some measures (i.e., defensive strikers spent less time hiding under shelter or remaining motionless outside shelter), they also made fewer quadrat transitions. Hybrids (C. scutulatus × viridis) did not exhibit syndromes involving defensive striking, but individuals that rattled more spent less time motionless outside the shelter in the open field test, but also made fewer quadrat transitions. Crotalus scutulatus, individuals with a higher propensity to strike during the threat assay spent more time in the hidebox in the open field test, and also made fewer quadrant transitions.
The behaviors we measured in captivity, although commonly used across a number of taxa, can be challenging to interpret in an ecological context. The time an individual spends in a hidebox or the time it spends motionless could indicate general activity level since spending more time in a hidebox is also time spent inactive. Similarly, spending more time in a hidebox could also reflect an unwillingness to perform a risky behavior (exposing oneself in an unfamiliar environment), or shyness. In our assay, individuals that spent more time motionless or in hideboxes were less likely to strike when threatened directly, so perhaps these measures are more indicative of general activity. However, this interpretation is complicated by the fact that it was necessary to handle snakes immediately prior to the exploration assays, perhaps leaving them in a stressed state. Future studies using alternative methods would be necessary to assess the relationship more closely between time spent motionless and bold, explorative, and active temperaments. Such studies may be able to resolve some apparent contradictions in our results between syndromes involving “explorativeness” and “defensiveness”. For example, in C. viridis striking (defensiveness) was negatively correlated with time spent in hidebox (low explorativeness), time spent motionless (low explorativeness), and the number of quadrant transitions (high explorativeness). In this case, it is possible that C. viridis that are more likely to strike defensively are more “anxious” individuals, and that personality trait is manifesting differently in different contexts. In the open field box test, individuals that were active but not crossing quadrants were often climbing at the walls of arena—perhaps analogous to escape or pacing behavior. Less anxious snakes would either move around to different hideboxes (quadrant crossing and hiding), or sit in place out in the open (time spent motionless). Untangling these intricacies would likely require repeated testing of individuals across several different behavioral contexts, most feasibly using a captive colony of snakes.
Contrary to several previous studies in other vertebrate taxa, we found that hybridization of these lineages did not lead to the breakdown of behavioral syndromes; rather, hybrids expressed syndromes within the same contexts as parental lineages (i.e., defensiveness, explorativeness), but involving different combinations of traits than either parental type. Although hybridization can generate novel or extreme traits18,20,76, it may be difficult to predict a priori how recombination and introgression would affect the trait combinations related to temperament and syndromes. More research is needed in order to determine if general patterns apply across taxonomic groups.
In addition to differences between parental and hybrid snakes, we also found that temperament and syndromes varied across age groups. Although these shifts in temperament across age classes have been found in other reptiles, they have not previously been confirmed in rattlesnakes77. Across all groups juveniles did not rattle as readily as adult snakes. These patterns could be driven by higher predation pressure on juveniles, given that their smaller size results in less effective antipredator behaviors (e.g., shorter effective strike range, smaller venom glands) and a higher likelihood of being killed by a wider variety of predators78. Following this logic, adult snakes could be more likely to rattle and draw attention to themselves because of their enhanced ability to effectively strike and envenomate potential predators.
We also found that juvenile snakes did not exhibit the same behavioral syndrome between defensiveness and explorativeness that was found in adults. This was not entirely unexpected and has been reported in other vertebrate taxa. Even in cases where temperaments of juveniles are quantifiable, considerable shifts in behavior occur with ontogeny10. Additionally, selection could also be eliminating maladaptive behavioral combinations and thus leading to convergence of traits into behavioral syndromes as animals age79.
While we found several behavioral measures were correlated across contexts in the laboratory assay, there was much more limited evidence for behavioral syndromes that spanned the laboratory assays and field-based behaviors (although our sample size for these comparisons was much more limited). For all adult snakes combined, we found a significant relationship between increased exploration in the laboratory assay (number of quadrant transitions) and the field (more frequent movements). This syndrome indicates that the temperament of an individual could affect metrics associated with spatial ecology, as has been found in other systems80, and be used by researchers to make broad (but tentative) generalizations about the spatial ecology of individuals that have been assayed under laboratory conditions.
Although this relationship makes intuitive sense, supporting evidence in our study is still limited. Most of the movement and exploratory behaviors we measured in the laboratory were not correlated with any of the behaviors measured when those snakes were free-ranging. This may be in part because the laboratory assays and field measures of behavior represent very different contexts. Compared to an animal’s home range in the field, the standardized arena in which animals are tested in an exploratory assay is small and devoid of sensory cues that would be typical of a natural setting. The high degree of variability in spatial behaviors expressed by free-ranging snakes is likely associated with the greater degree of biotic and abiotic variability across natural systems, and might make it difficult to detect consistent relationships across contexts.
It is also possible that behavioral temperaments and syndromes are species- or population-specific. The suite of behaviors we measured in the laboratory assays was based off of a study of a captive colony of Western Rattlesnakes (C. oreganus), which displayed individual repeatability for these behaviors40. Even though Western, Prairie, and Mojave Rattlesnakes are closely related and diverged recently (Prairie/Western: 5.24 MYA; Prairie/Mojave: 5.47 MYA43), it is possible that interspecific variation exists in the degree and type of behavioral temperaments and syndromes, and that repeatability of behaviors or existence of syndromes found in other species and populations are not present at our site. Thus, although our relatively short-term study was able to document a suite of behaviors correlated across contexts, future work should validate how robust these behavioral syndromes are over time in an effort to further understand their relevance to individual fitness and population persistence.
Although we did not find that hybrids had generally weaker behavioral syndromes than parentals, we did find that hybrids (but not parentals) that moved more in the open field arena were also significantly less likely to rattle during the handling test, a syndrome that could indicate a lack of risk aversion in some individuals. Because predation risk for snakes appears to be highest when they are moving between sites51, individuals that move more and are less likely to exhibit appropriate antipredator responses may be more at risk from predation. Of the 18 C. scutulatus × viridis that were implanted with transmitters, seven (38.9%) of them died before the end of the tracking season. Although we could only directly confirm one of these was killed by a predator, predation is a likely source of mortality for the other individuals45. Qualitatively, this was a higher level of mortality than we observed for C. scutulatus (one out of 21, or 4.8% mortality) and C. viridis (two out of 17, or 11.8%)45. Hybrid individuals that tend to rattle less but moved more could be subject to higher levels of predation due to increased numbers of encounters and lower probability of engaging in effective anti-predator displays. However, more directed behavioral experiments paired with a long-term mark-recapture program would be necessary to quantitatively test the hypothesis that C. scutulatus × viridis are more susceptible to predation.
In conclusion, although the rattlesnakes we studied in this hybrid zone were broadly similar in temperament and syndromes, we did identify a few key differences that could potentially play a role in hybridization dynamics. Unexpectedly, we did not find significantly more variability in temperament within the hybrid lineage when compared to parentals. Nevertheless, this study is one of a few that integrates laboratory-based behavioral assays with behaviors expressed under natural conditions. This integration is extremely important for developing a holistic view of the utility of using data collected in captivity to understand natural populations. Future research could build on these findings by developing focused approaches to understand the role of temperament and syndromes in shaping survival and fitness, or reproductive isolation between lineages. Such efforts could yield important findings on the subtleties of how closely related species differ from each other, and how these differences affect reproductive isolation and the potential for speciation.
Methods
Study sites
The hybrid zone is located within the Cochise Filter Barrier (CFB)[Figure 1 in 46, a transitional region between the Chihuahuan and Sonoran deserts in the southwestern USA that is frequently implicated in lineage divergence associated with climatic and vegetation community shifts induced by glacial cycling81. Because there is not a major physical barrier separating the two deserts, the CFB is a region of frequent gene flow and hybridization between genetic groups of organisms82,83. The hybrid zone between Crotalus scutulatus and C. viridis occupies a valley between the Peloncillo and Animas Mountains in the extreme southwest of New Mexico, USA. Hybrid snakes are found in a narrow band (~ 12 km) of mosaic habitat in the center of the valley, an area defined by the transition from creosote lowland desert to the southwest to short-grass prairie to the northeast. Parental populations were located on either side of the bordering mountain ranges84. For further details on the hybrid zone, refer to46.
Snake sampling and surgical procedures
We collected and sampled all rattlesnakes encountered via road and visual encounter surveys within and adjacent to the hybrid zone from 2019–2021[Figure 1 in 45. Capture, processing, and field telemetry/videography methods followed previous work45,46. In summary individuals were captured in the field, transported to the field station, sexed via hemipene probing, measured for a suite of morphometric characters, categorized as adult or juvenile (based off body size at initial capture and later confirmed using the distribution of Scaled Mass Index detailed in46; Figure S.3), tested for their laboratory behaviors, and either released at their initial point of capture or held for surgical implantation of radio transmitters and then released at their initial point of capture. All procedures were approved by the San Diego State University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (APF# 22-07-008 C), and animals were collected via a New Mexico Department of Game and Fish Scientific Collection permit (authorization number 3605). All procedures were also performed in accordance with ARRIVE guidelines: to reduce the number of animals involved in the experiment, animals were used as their own control; outcome measures are clearly defined; statistical analysis is fully detailed and the code is available on the following Dryad repository: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.5tb2rbpbd; the species, sex and age of animals are reported; and the experimental procedures are described in detail. All the methods were carried out in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations.
We tested every subject for defensive and exploratory behaviors within 48 h of their initial capture. All testing was at 22–26 °C to minimize variation in behavior owing to body temperature. This temperature range is well within the range of naturally occurring temperatures at the hybrid zone, and overlaps with the field-active body temperature of actively hunting rattlesnakes85. Individuals were given at least four hours to acclimate to the room’s temperature prior to conducting tests. We followed the procedures in Gibert et al.40for conducting and scoring handling, exploratory, and threat assays. Snakes were assayed in a 1.2 × 1.2 × 1.2 m four-walled arena constructed from polyvinyl sheeting and dimly lit with indirect light. We divided the floor of the arena into four equal quadrants using black tape and placed a 10 × 5 × 3 cm black hidebox within each quadrant so that they were equidistance from each other and the center of the arena[Figure 1 in 40). After testing each snake, we cleaned the entire arena using a commercial cleaner (Lysol®) and the hideboxes with soap and water. We recorded each of the three assays (handling, exploratory, and threat) with a Sony Handycam® (model DCR-SR80) mounted above the arena. When snakes were not being tested they were housed in their own cage, provided a hide box, and were given water ad-libitum.
Handling assay
To assay defensive rattling, we simulated a predator attack by removing snakes with 1 m long metal tongs (Midwest Tongs®) from their temporary holding container after the 4-hour acclimation period and held them 1 m above the center of the arena at midbody for 30 s. At the 15 s mark we gently shook the snake back and forth twice. Once 30 s had elapsed, we released the snake in the center of the arena and left the area, starting the exploratory assay. Whether or not the snake rattled defensively during the handling assay was recorded from the video footage.
Exploratory assay
Snakes were left to explore the arena undisturbed for 60 min. We then reviewed the video footage after each assay to quantify the duration of time the snake spent in a hidebox, the number of transitions between quadrants, and the duration it spent motionless outside of a hidebox. We considered the snake to have entered a hidebox or a new quadrant when the entire front half of the individual was either obscured from view within the hidebox or had broken the plane created by the black tape. We considered the snake to be motionless when no detectable movement of its head, body, or tail could be seen or heard.
Threat assay
After the conclusion of the exploratory assay, we turned on an overhead light to fully illuminate the arena and, using metal tongs, removed all hideboxes and moved the snake into one corner of the arena. We then “threatened” the snake with an inflated balloon, around 0.25 m in diameter, on the end of another pair of metal tongs to create a visual, looming stimulus. For each assay the balloon tong was raised about 0.25 m into the air and tapped in the center of the floor of the arena five times, around 80 cm from the center of the snake with each tap taking around 1 s. We then slid the balloon along the floor of the arena and towards the head of the snake until it was around 40 cm from the snake. We then tapped the balloon tong on the floor of the arena another five times. After the initial tapping, we slid the balloon along the floor of the arena again until it touched the snout of the snake. If the snake did not strike at this point of the assay, we simulated a more immediate threat by tapping the balloon on the head of the snake five times. If at any point the snake struck at the balloon (even if the snake missed) the assay would end, and a positive strike response would be reported for that snake. Otherwise, the assay would end on the fifth tap of the balloon on the snake’s head and a negative response would be recorded, usually taking around 20 s. The threat assay was only performed on snakes captured during the 2020 and 2021 active seasons.
Genetic assignments of individuals to parental species or hybrids
To assign snakes as parental or hybrid individuals we analyzed reduced-representation genomic data obtained from double-digest RAD sequencing (ddRADseq) and whole-genome sequencing. All sequenced samples were mapped to the C. viridis reference genome43. For individuals that had their whole genome sequenced, genomic data were downsampled to only include loci that overlapped with ddRADseq loci. Hybrid index (HI—proportion of the genome derived from C. viridis) scores for individuals were inferred across individuals using ADMIXTURE with K = 2. We classified any snake with an HI between 5 and 95% as a hybrid (see Fig. 2 in46 or Figure S4). Due to extraction or sequencing failures, we were not able to obtain HI estimates for six of the 185 snakes analyzed for their temperament and syndromes (based on face mask and tail banding patterns, and geography, one putative C. scutulatus, three putative C. viridis, and two putative C. scutulatus × viridis). We re-ran all analyses with and without these individuals and found no difference in the overall patterns (see Tables S1–S3 for analyses without these individuals), and thus chose to report the results that included the individuals with assignments based solely on morphology (Figure S5) and geographic locale.
Statistical analyses
Behavioral assays were scored independently by two different observers blind to snake identity to minimize observer bias. If scores were similar (values within 15%), we averaged the scores of the two observers, but if there was a greater than 15% difference between observer scores, the video was scored by a third observer to achieve a consensus score (average of the two scores with < 15% difference).
To assess temperament and potential syndromes, we limited analyses to the behaviors that were found to be repeatable in previous research assessing repeatability of temperament traits in C. oreganus40. The traits identified by Gibert et al.40are also broadly similar to other temperament traits that have been found to be individually repeatable across a broad sample of snakes, lizards, and other vertebrates86 (including the conspecific C. atrox41). Thus, we scored rattling behavior (whether snake rattled or not during the handling assay) and defensive strike behavior (whether snake struck or not during the threat assay) as traits indicative of defensive/passive temperaments77,87,88,89,90,91. During the exploratory test, we scored the proportion of time a snake spent in a hidebox, the proportion of time spent motionless (not moving, but not in hidebox), and the number of times the snake transitioned to a new quadrant. Behaviors like these displayed in open-field tests are typically considered indicative of explorative/non-explorative temperaments4,92.
We created a suite of models using a Generalized Linear Model (GLM) framework to assess variation in defensive and exploratory temperaments across genetic groups (C. scutulatus, C. viridis, and C. scutulatus × viridis). Four models were constructed for each of the response variables (defensive rattling, proportion of time spent in a hidebox, number of quadrant transitions, proportion of time spent motionless, and striking), with each modeling the predictor variables of genetic group, group + age (adult or juvenile), group * age, and group * age + sex. We then used Akaike information criterion (AICC) to select which of the four models best fit the data. If more than one model was within two ΔAICCof the best model, then we used the simplest model for analysis (the model with fewest predictor variables). We used a binomial distribution for modelling rattling, striking, and proportion of time motionless, a beta distribution for proportion of time in hidebox, and a negative binomial distribution for the number of quadrant transitions (overdispersed count data). Time spent in hidebox was zero-inflated, so we performed the transformation recommended by Smithson and Verkuilen93. We used Tukey tests for post-hoc comparisons and Levene’s test to compare variation between groups in exploratory behaviors. To assess if there was a relationship between the value of the hybrid index and temperament, we repeated the analyses using only hybrid snakes and incorporating HI as a fixed factor.
Due to non-normality of predictor variables and the binary nature of response variables, we were not able to use a correlation framework for analyzing behavioral syndromes between defensiveness and explorativeness, and instead used GLMs to test for significant relationships between behavioral traits within each group15,40,73,94. For each group, we constructed binomial GLMs with the three exploratory behaviors as the fixed factors and either rattling or defensive striking as the response variable. Lastly, if any previous models indicated age was a significant factor, we constructed independent models for adults and juveniles.
We also assessed potential syndromes between behaviors measured in laboratory assays and movement and hunting behaviors measured while individuals were free-ranging. These analyses were constrained in sample size, as we could only include the subset of radio-tagged individuals with enough spatial and hunting data for statistical analyses. These snakes were captured, implanted with radio-transmitters, released back at their point of capture and tracked for 1–2 active seasons. Each time the snake was found a GPS point was taken for spatial ecology/movement analysis. If the snake was found hunting, then a fixed field-videography set-up was deployed to document the entire hunting event for hunting behavior and diet analysis45. For this analysis we assessed relationships between explorative behaviors measured in captive assays and a suite of behaviors related to space use and foraging that we quantified in previous studies45,46, including: average distance moved per day (DMD), average number of days between movements (FM), patchiness of space use (number of 50% isopleths identified by Brownian Bridge Kernel Density Estimators (bbKDE), rate of chemosensory probing (NPR), prey encounter frequency (PE), time of day of hunting site abandonment (AT), and the frequency of nights that the snakes were found hunting (HF). We performed a Spearman Correlation test on the spatial and hunting behaviors and each of the three exploratory behaviors for each group and (in order to maximize sample size) for all groups combined. We adjusted p-values with a Holm’s adjustment to account for multiple comparisons. Following others65,70,95, we considered a syndrome to exist between two behaviors if the correlation between them was significant and if |r| > 0.3.
All behavioral scoring was done from the video footage post-hoc using BORIS v. 7.4.1196. All statistical analyses were done in R v. 3.6.397using the packages tidyverse98, Hmisc99, nortest100, psych101, betareg102, ggplot2103, emmeans104, MuMIn105, performance106.
Data availability
Raw sequence reads for RADseq data are available under the NCBI SRA (sequence read archive) BioProjectID number: PRJNA1010815. The final VCF alignment file used for hybrid index analysis; individual behavior scores are available on Dryad: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.5tb2rbpbd.
References
MacKinlay, R. D. & Shaw, R. C. A systematic review of animal personality in conservation science. Conserv. Biol. e13935 https://doi.org/10.1111/cobi.13935 (2022).
Bell, A. M., Hankison, S. J. & Laskowski, K. L. The repeatability of behaviour: a meta-analysis. Anim. Behav. 77, 771–783 (2009).
van Oers, K., de Jong, G., van Noordwijk, A. J., Kempenaers, B. & Drent, P. J. Contribution of genetics to the study of animal personalities: a review of case studies. Behaviour 142, 1185–1206 (2005).
Carter, A. J., Feeney, W. E., Marshall, H. H., Cowlishaw, G. & Heinsohn, R. Animal personality: what are behavioural ecologists measuring? Biol. Rev. 88, 465–475 (2013).
Sih, A., Bell, A. M., Johnson, J. C. & Ziemba, R. E. Behavioral syndromes: an integrative overview. Q. Rev. Biol. 79, 241–277 (2004).
Dingemanse, N. J., Kazem, A. J. N., Réale, D. & Wright, J. Behavioural reaction norms: animal personality Meets individual plasticity. Trends Ecol. Evol. 25, 81–89 (2010).
Biro, P. A. & Stamps, J. A. Are animal personality traits linked to life-history productivity? Trends Ecol. Evol. 23, 361–368 (2008).
Schuett, W., Tregenza, T. & Dall, S. R. X. Sexual selection and animal personality. Biol. Rev. 85, 217–246 (2010).
Sih, A. et al. Animal personality and state-behaviour feedbacks: a review and guide for empiricists. Trends Ecol. Evol. 30, 50–60 (2015).
Cabrera, D., Nilsson, J. R. & Griffen, B. D. The development of animal personality across ontogeny: a cross-species review. Anim. Behav. 173, 137–144 (2021).
Laskowski, K. L., Chang, C. C. & Sheehy, K. Aguin Tild Aga, J. Consistent individual behavioral variation: what do we know and where are we going? Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 53, 161–182 (2022).
Rieseberg, L. H. et al. Hybridization and the colonization of novel habitats by annual sunflowers. Genetica 129, 149–165 (2007).
Simon, A., Bierne, N. & Welch, J. J. Coadapted genomes and selection on hybrids: Fisher’s geometric model explains a variety of empirical patterns. Evol. Lett. 2, 472–498 (2018).
Horta-Lacueva, Q. J. B., Benhaïm, D., Morrissey, M. B., Snorrason, S. S. & Kapralova, K. H. Animal personality adds complexity to the processes of divergence between sympatric morphs of Arctic Charr. Anim. Behav. 175, 57–73 (2021).
Johnson, J. B., Culumber, Z. W., Easterling, R. & Rosenthal, G. G. Boldness and predator evasion in naturally hybridizing swordtails (Teleostei: Xiphophorus). Curr. Zool. 61, 596–603 (2015).
Islam, S. S., Wringe, B. F., Bradbury, I. R. & Fleming, I. A. Behavioural variation among divergent European and North American farmed and wild Atlantic salmon (Salmo salar) populations. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 230, 105029 (2020).
Hosoya, S., Suetake, H., Suzuki, Y. & Kikuchi, K. Genetic basis underlying behavioral correlation between Fugu Takifugu rubripes and a closely related species, Takifugu niphobles. Behav. Genet. 45, 560–572 (2015).
Rieseberg, L. H., Archer, M. A. & Wayne, R. K. Transgressive segregation, adaptation and speciation. Heredity (Edinb). 83, 363–372 (1999).
Stelkens, R. & Seehausen, O. Genetic distance between species predicts novel trait expression in their hybrids. Evol. (N Y). 63, 884–897 (2009).
Harrison, R. G. & Larson, E. L. Hybridization, introgression, and the nature of species boundaries. J. Hered. 105, 795–809 (2014).
Seehausen, O. Hybridization and adaptive radiation. Trends Ecol. Evol. 19, 198–207 (2004).
Ingley, S. J. & Johnson, J. B. Animal personality as a driver of reproductive isolation. Trends Ecol. Evol. 29, 369–371 (2014).
Harcourt, J. L., Sweetman, G., Johnstone, R. A. & Manica, A. Personality counts: the effect of boldness on shoal choice in Three-spined sticklebacks. Anim. Behav. 77, 1501–1505 (2009).
Michelangeli, M., Wong, B. B. M. & Chapple, D. G. It’s a trap: sampling bias due to animal personality is not always inevitable. Behav. Ecol. 27, 62–67 (2016).
Wilson, A. D. M. & Godin, J. G. J. Boldness and behavioral syndromes in the Bluegill sunfish, Lepomis macrochirus. Behav. Ecol. 20, 231–237 (2009).
Sinn, D. L. & Moltschaniwskyj, N. A. Personality traits in dumpling squid (Euprymna tasmanica): context-specific traits and their correlation with biological characteristics. J. Comp. Psychol. 119, 99–110 (2005).
Reaney, L. T. & Backwell, P. R. Y. Risk-taking behavior predicts aggression and mating success in a fiddler crab. Behav. Ecol. 18, 521–525 (2007).
Breck, S. W., Poessel, S. A., Mahoney, P. & Young, J. K. The intrepid urban Coyote: a comparison of bold and exploratory behavior in Coyotes from urban and rural environments. Sci. Rep. 9, 2104 (2019).
del Mar Delgado, M. & Penteriani, V. Behavioral States help translate dispersal movements into Spatial distribution patterns of floaters. Am. Nat. 172, 475–485 (2008).
Fraser, D. F., Gilliam, J. F., Daley, M. J., Le, A. N. & Skalski, G. T. Explaining leptokurtic movement distributions: intrapopulation variation in boldness and exploration. Am. Nat. 158, 124–135 (2001).
Toscano, B. J., Gownaris, N. J., Heerhartz, S. M. & Monaco, C. J. Personality, foraging behavior and specialization: integrating behavioral and food web ecology at the individual level. Oecologia 182, 55–69 (2016).
Stuber, E. F., Carlson, B. S. & Jesmer, B. R. Spatial personalities: a meta-analysis of consistent individual differences in Spatial behavior. Behav. Ecol. 33, 477–486 (2022).
Sinn, D. L., Apiolaza, L. A. & Moltschaniwskyj, N. A. Heritability and fitness-related consequences of squid personality traits. J. Evol. Biol. 19, 1437–1447 (2006).
Wilson, A. D. M. & Stevens, E. D. Consistency in context-specific measures of shyness and boldness in rainbow trout, Oncorhynchus mykiss. Ethology 111, 849–862 (2005).
Quinn, J. L. & Cresswell, W. Personality, anti-predation behaviour and behavioural plasticity in the chaffinch Fringilla coelebs. Behaviour 142, 1377–1402 (2005).
Réale, D. & Festa-Bianchet, M. Predator-induced natural selection on temperament in Bighorn Ewes. Anim. Behav. 65, 463–470 (2003).
Stapley, J. & Keogh, J. S. Behavioral syndromes influence mating systems: floater pairs of a Lizard have heavier offspring. Behav. Ecol. 16, 514–520 (2005).
Sinn, D. L., Gosling, S. D. & Moltschaniwskyj, N. A. Development of Shy/bold behaviour in Squid: context-specific phenotypes associated with developmental plasticity. Anim. Behav. 75, 433–442 (2008).
Collins, S. M. et al. Bibliometric investigation of the integration of animal personality in conservation contexts. Conserv. Biol. e14021. https://doi.org/10.1111/cobi.COBI14021 (2022).
Gibert, R. G., Maag, D. W., Sanders, L. N. & Clark, R. W. Investigating personality in vipers: individual rattlesnakes exhibit consistent behavioral responses in defensive and exploratory contexts. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 76, 132 (2022).
Da Cunha, O., Horne, L. M. & Johnson, J. D. Personally rattled: a unique protocol to support the presence of personality and behavioral syndromes in rattlesnakes. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 77, 115 (2023).
Schield, D. R. et al. Cryptic genetic diversity, population structure, and gene flow in the Mojave rattlesnake (Crotalus scutulatus). Mol. Phylogenet Evol. 127, 669–681 (2018).
Schield, D. R. et al. Allopatric divergence and secondary contact with gene flow: a recurring theme in rattlesnake speciation. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 128, 149–169 (2019).
Nikolakis, Z. L. et al. Evidence that genomic incompatibilities and other multilocus processes impact hybrid fitness in a rattlesnake hybrid zone. Evol. (N Y). 76, 2513–2530 (2022).
Maag, D. W., Francioli, Y. Z., Castoe, T. A., Schuett, G. W. & Clark, R. W. The spatial ecology of Mojave Rattlesnakes (Crotalus scutulatus), Prairie Rattlesnakes (C. viridis), and their hybrids in southwestern New Mexico. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. blae037 (2024).
Maag, D. W. et al. Hunting behavior and feeding ecology of Mojave rattlesnakes (Crotalus scutulatus), prairie rattlesnakes (C. viridis), and their hybrids in Southwestern new Mexico. Ecol. Evol. 13, e10683 (2023).
Ballew, N. G., Mittelbach, G. G. & Scribner, K. T. Fitness consequences of boldness in juvenile and adult largemouth bass. Am. Nat. 189, 396–406 (2017).
Smith, B. R. & Blumstein, D. T. Fitness consequences of personality: a meta-analysis. Behav. Ecol. 19, 448–455 (2008).
von Merten, S., Dingemanse, N. J., Mathias, M. L. & Rychlik, L. Individual behavior, behavioral stability, and Pace of life within and among five shrew species. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 74, 15 (2020).
Klauber, L. M. The Rattle. in Rattlesnakes: their habits, life histories, and influence on mankind 249–322 (1956).
Maag, D. & Clark, R. Safety in coils: predation rates of ambush hunting rattlesnakes are extremely low. Amphib Reptil. 43, 425–430 (2022).
Kashon, E. A. F. & Carlson, B. E. Consistently bolder turtles maintain higher body temperatures in the field but May experience greater predation risk. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 72, 9 (2018).
Carter, A. J., Goldizen, A. W. & Tromp, S. A. Agamas exhibit behavioral syndromes: bolder males Bask and feed more but May suffer higher predation. Behav. Ecol. 21, 655–661 (2010).
Bell, A. M. & Sih, A. Exposure to predation generates personality in threespined sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus). Ecol. Lett. 10, 828–834 (2007).
Guzmán-Novoa, E., Hunt, G. J., Uribe, J. L., Smith, C. & Arechavaleta-Velasco, M. E. Confirmation of QTL effects and evidence of genetic dominance of honeybee defensive behavior: results of colony and individual behavioral assays. Behav. Genet. 32, 96–102 (2002).
Nakayama, S., Nishi, Y. & Miyatake, T. Genetic correlation between behavioural traits in relation to death-feigning behaviour. Popul. Ecol. 52, 329–335 (2010).
Tay, N. E., Warburton, N. M., Moseby, K. E. & Fleming, P. A. Predator escape behaviour in threatened marsupials. Anim. Conserv. 1–14. https://doi.org/10.1111/acv.12847 (2023).
Gammie, S. C., Garland, T. & Stevenson, S. A. Artificial selection for increased maternal defense behavior in mice. Behav. Genet. 36, 713–722 (2006).
Kim, S. Y. & Velando, A. Phenotypic integration between antipredator behavior and camouflage pattern in juvenile sticklebacks. Evol. (N Y). 69, 830–838 (2015).
Satterfield, D. & Johnson, D. W. Local adaptation of antipredator behaviors in populations of a temperate reef fish. Oecologia 194, 571–584 (2020).
Jiang, Y. & Møller, A. P. Escape from predators and genetic variance in birds. J. Evol. Biol. 30, 2059–2067 (2017).
Bize, P., Diaz, C. & Lindström, J. Experimental evidence that adult antipredator behaviour is heritable and not influenced by behavioural copying in a wild bird. Proc. R Soc. B Biol. Sci. 279, 1380–1388 (2012).
Baxter-Gilbert, J., Riley, J. L. & Whiting, M. J. Runners and fighters: clutch effects and body size drive innate antipredator behaviour in hatchling lizards. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 72, 97 (2018).
Michelangeli, M., Chapple, D. G., Goulet, C. T., Bertram, M. G. & Wong, B. M. Behavioral syndromes vary among geographically distinct populations in a reptile. Behav. Ecol. 30, 393–401 (2019).
Lukas, J. et al. Consistent behavioral syndrome across seasons in an invasive freshwater fish. Front. Ecol. Evol. 8, 583670 (2021).
Kudo, H., Nishizawa, H., Uchida, K. & Sato, K. Boldness–exploration behavioral syndrome in wild sub-adult green sea turtles caught at Oita, Japan. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 236, 105216 (2021).
Xu, W. et al. Environmental complexity during early life shapes average behavior in adulthood. Behav. Ecol. 32, 105–113 (2021).
Schabacker, T. et al. In situ novel environment assay reveals acoustic exploration as a repeatable behavioral response in migratory bats. Sci. Rep. 11, 8174 (2021).
Majelantle, T. L. et al. Aggression, boldness, and exploration personality traits in the subterranean naked Mole-Rat (Heterocephalus glaber) disperser morphs. Animals 12, 3083 (2022).
Nyqvist, M. J., Gozlan, R. E., Cucherousset, J. & Britton, J. R. Behavioural syndrome in a solitary predator is independent of body size and growth rate. PLoS One. 7, e31619 (2012).
Muraco, J. J., Monroe, D. J., Aspbury, A. S. & Gabor, C. R. Do females in a unisexual-bisexual species complex differ in their behavioral syndromes and cortisol production? Biology (Basel). 10, 186 (2021).
Ferderer, A., Davis, A. R. & Wong, M. Y. L. Temperature and body size influence personality and behavioural syndromes in an invasive crayfish. Anim. Behav. 190, 187–198 (2022).
Dhellemmes, F., Finger, J. S., Laskowski, K. L., Guttridge, T. L. & Krause, J. Comparing behavioural syndromes across time and ecological conditions in a free-ranging predator. Anim. Behav. 162, 23–33 (2020).
Agnani, P., Thomson, J., Schradin, C. & Careau, V. The fast and the curious II: performance, personality, and metabolism in Karoo Bush rats. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 74, 123 (2020).
Underhill, V. et al. Personality and behavioral syndromes in two peromyscus species: presence, lack of state dependence, and lack of association with home range size. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 75, 9 (2021).
Stelkens, R. B., Schmid, C., Selz, O. & Seehausen, O. Phenotypic novelty in experimental hybrids is predicted by the genetic distance between species of cichlid fish. BMC Evol. Biol. 9, 283 (2009).
Simkova, O., Frýdlová, P., Zampachová, B., Frynta, D. & Landová, E. Development of behavioural profile in the Northern common Boa (Boa imperator): repeatable independent traits or personality? PLoS ONE 12, e0177911 (2017).
Klauber, L. M. Enemies of rattlesnakes. in Rattlesnakes: their Habits, Life Histories, and Influence on Mankind 1064–1115 (University of California Press, Ltd., 1956).
Adriaenssens, B. & Johnsson, J. I. Natural selection, plasticity and the emergence of a behavioural syndrome in the wild. Ecol. Lett. 16, 47–55 (2013).
Spiegel, O., Leu, S. T., Bull, C. M. & Sih, A. What’s your move? Movement as a link between personality and Spatial dynamics in animal populations. Ecol. Lett. 20, 3–18 (2017).
Van Devender, T. R., Betancourt, J. L. & Wimberly, M. Biogeographic implications of a packrat midden sequence from the Sacramento mountains, south-central new Mexico. Quat Res. 22, 344–360 (1984).
Pyron, R. A. & Burbrink, F. T. Hard and soft allopatry: physically and ecologically mediated modes of geographic speciation. J. Biogeogr. 37, 2005–2015 (2010).
Castoe, T. A., Spencer, C. L. & Parkinson, C. L. Phylogeographic structure and historical demography of the Western Diamondback rattlesnake (Crotalus atrox): A perspective on North American desert biogeography. Mol. Phylogenet Evol. 42, 193–212 (2007).
Zancolli, G. et al. Is hybridization a source of adaptive venom. Toxins (Basel). 8, 188 (2016).
Putman, B. J. & Clark, R. W. Behavioral thermal tolerances of free-ranging rattlesnakes (Crotalus oreganus) during the summer foraging season. J. Therm. Biol. 65, 8–15 (2017).
Waters, M. R., Bowers, B. B. & Burghardt, G. M. Personality and individuality in reptile behavior. In Personality in Nonhuman Animals (eds Vonk, J. et al.) 153–184 (Springer, 2017).
Maillet, Z., Halliday, W. D. & Blouin-Demers, G. Exploratory and defensive behaviours change with sex and body size in Eastern garter snakes (Thamnophis sirtalis). J. Ethol. 33, 47–54 (2015).
Scudder, R. M. & Burghardt, G. M. A comparative study of defensive behavior in three sympatric species of water snakes (Nerodia). Z. Tierpsychol. 63, 17–26 (1983).
Goode, M. J. & Duvall, D. Body temperature and defensive behaviour of free-ranging prairie rattlesnakes, Crotalus viridis viridis. Anim. Behav. 38, 360–362 (1988).
Arnold, S. J. & Bennett, A. F. Behavioural variation in natural populations. III: antipredator displays in the garter snake Thamnophis radix. Anim. Behav. 32, 1108–1118 (1984).
Herzog, H. A., Bowers, B. B. & Burghardt, G. M. Stimulus control of antipredator behavior in newborn and juvenile garter snakes (Thamnophis). J. Comp. Psychol. 103, 233–242 (1989).
Perals, D., Griffin, A. S., Bartomeus, I. & Sol, D. Revisiting the open-field test: what does it really tell Us about animal personality? Anim. Behav. 123, 69–79 (2017).
Smithson, M. & Verkuilen, J. A better lemon squeezer? Maximum-likelihood regression with beta-distributed dependent variables. Psychol. Methods. 11, 54–71 (2006).
De Meester, G., Pafilis, P. & Van Damme, R. Bold and Bright: shy and supple? The effect of habitat type on personality–cognition covariance in The Aegean wall Lizard (Podarcis erhardii). Anim. Cogn. 25, 745–767 (2022).
Pruitt, J. N. et al. Population differences in behaviour are explained by shared within-population trait correlations. J. Evol. Biol. 23, 748–756 (2010).
Friard, O. & Gamba, M. BORIS: a free, versatile open-source event-logging software for video/audio coding and live observations. Methods Ecol. Evol. 7, 1325–1330 (2016).
R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing (2021).
Wickham, H. et al. Welcome to the {tidyverse}. J. Open. Source Softw. 4, 1686 (2019).
Harrell, F. E. Jr With Contributions from Charles Dupont & Many Others (Harrell Miscellaneous, 2021).
Gross, J. & Ligges, U. nortest: Tests for normality (2015).
Revelle, W. psych: Procedures for Personality and Psychological Research (2021).
Francisco, C. N. & Zeileis, A. Beta regression in R. J. Stat. Softw. 34, 1–24 (2010).
Wickham, H. ggplot2: elegant graphics for data analysis (2016).
Lenth, R. V. Emmeans: estimated marginal means, aka least-squares means. https://doi.org/10.1080/00031305.1980.10483031>License (2021).
Barton, K. & MuMIn Multi-Model Inference (2020).
Ludeck, D., Ben-Shachar, M. S., Patil, I., Waggoner, P. & Makowski, D. performance: and R package for assessment, comparison and testing of statistical models. https://doi.org/10.21105/joss.03139 (2021).
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank the following field technicians for helping acquire the field data for this research: Joshua Benavidez, Nicholas Dunham, Diana Ouk, Ricardo Galaz Gibert, Kaitlyn Wiese, Nathaniel Cornille, Emily Allen, Matthew Goetz, Elizabeth Boulanger, Megan Quinn, Lynn Jacobs, Alexander Megerle, Andressa Viol, and Jeffrey Ferrer. We thank Bob and Sheri Ashley and the staff of the Chiricahua Desert Museum for providing logistical support during the field collection seasons. We also thank Bill Cavaliere, the Kerr family, Everett and Leah Ashearst, Mike and Joan Tracey, Jim Walter, Mark Thomas, and Bob Ashley for granting us access to private properties. We thank Drs. Polly Campbell, Timothy Higham, and Marshal Hedin for their insights and advice during the development, collection of data, and preparation of this manuscript. This research was funded by grants to DWM from the Society for the Study of Amphibians and Reptiles (Roger Conant Grant in Herpetology), the Chiricahua Desert Museum (Charles W. Painter Grant in Herpetology), the American Society of Ichthyologists and Herpetologists (Helen T. and Frederick M. Gaige Grant), the Southwestern Center for Herpetological Research (Klauber Summer Research Grant), the University of California, Riverside (Irwin M. Newell Graduate Research Fund; Graduate Research Award; Herman T. and Evelyn W. Spieth Memorial Graduate Award), and San Diego State University (Completion of Research and Creative Activity Fellowship). Additional support came from NSF IOS-1856404 to RWC.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
D.M.: Conceptualization (lead); Data Curation (lead); Formal Analysis (lead); Funding Acquisition (equal); Investigation (lead); Methodology (lead); Project Administration (equal); Resources (lead); Software (lead); Supervision (equal); Validation (lead); Visualization (lead); Writing – original draft (lead); Writing – review and editing (equal). Y.F.: Data Acquisition (supporting), Data Analysis (supporting), Data Curation (supporting), Writing – original draft (supporting); Writing – review and editing (supporting). M.G.: Data Curation (supporting); Investigation (supporting); Writing – review and editing (supporting). L.S.: Data Curation (supporting); Investigation (supporting); Writing – review and editing (supporting). X.L.: Data Curation (supporting); Investigation (supporting); Writing – review and editing (supporting). T.C.: Data Acquisition (supporting), Data Analysis (supporting), Writing – original draft (supporting); Writing – review and editing (supporting). G.S.: Conceptualization (supporting); Methodology (supporting); Project Administration (supporting); Data Acquisition (supporting); Writing – review and editing (supporting). and R.C.: Conceptualization (supporting); Funding Acquisition (equal); Investigation (supporting); Methodology (supporting); Project Administration (equal); Data Acquisition (supporting); Data Analysis (supporting); Supervision (equal); Visualization (supporting); Writing – original draft (supporting); Writing – review and editing (equal).
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher’s note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Electronic supplementary material
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Maag, D.W., Francioli, Y.Z., Goetz, M.T.H. et al. Variation in defensive and exploratory behaviors across a rattlesnake (Crotalus scutulatus × viridis) hybrid zone in Southwestern new Mexico. Sci Rep 15, 11989 (2025). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-025-96155-8
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-025-96155-8