Your privacy, your choice

We use essential cookies to make sure the site can function. We also use optional cookies for advertising, personalisation of content, usage analysis, and social media.

By accepting optional cookies, you consent to the processing of your personal data - including transfers to third parties. Some third parties are outside of the European Economic Area, with varying standards of data protection.

See our privacy policy for more information on the use of your personal data.

for further information and to change your choices.

Skip to main content

Impact of Dark Triad personality traits on COVID-19 vaccination uptake and prevention efforts: insights from the European Covid Survey (ECOS)

Abstract

Background

Even though the COVID-19 vaccination roll-out in general can be considered as one of the most successful public health campaigns in the history of medicine, general vaccination hesitancy has remained an issue of concern throughout the world. We add to a deeper understanding of vaccination hesitancy by identifying what drives primary vaccination and booster uptake, as well as adherence to simple preventive measures such as physical distancing by investigating the role of Dark Triad personality traits, i.e. Machiavellianism, narcissism, and psychopathy.

Methods

We investigate data from Germany and the United Kingdom from the European Covid Survey which was collected from 23 December 2021 to 10 January 2022. Logit regressions and random effects regressions were performed to study the effect of dark personality traits on COVID-19-related prevention.

Results

We find a statistically significant association between Dark Triad personality traits and prevention efforts, primary vaccinations, and booster uptake against COVID-19. Specifically, individuals scoring high in psychopathy are associated with a lower likelihood of having received primary immunization. The marginal effect amounts to 3.31%-points. High narcissistic personality traits are correlated with a substantially higher likelihood (4.52%-points) to refuse booster shots after having received the primary vaccinations. Dark Triad personality traits may be relevant factors associated with vaccine-related decision-making. In addition, individuals with higher psychopathic tendencies report significantly lower engagement in other simple preventive behavior, while higher scores in narcissism are associated with higher reported adherence to simple preventive measures.

Conclusions

Our findings highlight the crucial role that personality plays in pandemic-related prevention. Policymakers, health professionals, and those in charge of health messaging may take these factors into account when devising communication strategies to improve the vaccination uptake and adherence to preventive behaviors. Future pandemics and public health crises would benefit from targeted, nuanced approaches to public health messaging to promote greater public adherence and public health.

Peer Review reports

Introduction

Vaccination hesitancy is a global phenomenon where a substantial part of the population is not getting vaccinated despite having access to the vaccine. Even though the COVID-19 vaccination roll-out in general can be considered as one of the most successful public health campaigns in the history of medicine, general vaccination hesitancy has remained an issue of concern throughout the world [1, 2], even within high risk groups such as pregnant women [3, 4]. The World Health Organization reports about 5.2 billion of the overall 7.8 billion global population have received full primary immunization for COVID-19 as of the end of the year 2023.

The decision to get a vaccination is a prevention decision, which requires an investment upfront (the actual vaccination and potential side effects thereafter) in the expectation of the long-term benefit of immunity as theoretically discussed by Courbage and Peter [5]. The fear of future side effects is one of the highest ranked reasons for hesitating to get vaccinated [6, 7]. Science has provided conclusive evidence that (public) health benefits by far exceed adverse outcomes from vaccinations. Nevertheless, individual preferences and behavioral biases may imply that individuals prefer not to get vaccinated or take protective action, as suggested by Campos-Mercade et al. [8], Müller and Rau [9], Zettler et al. [10] or Ścigała et al. [11]. In addition to the initial COVID-19 vaccination hesitancy among some groups within the population, also a sizeable number of individuals opted out from booster shots who had received primary vaccinations. The necessity for regular booster shots only became evident over time after promising results on vaccine efficiency at the end of 2021 [12]. New variants emerged and evidence showed that booster shots were required for long-term prevention as antibodies reduce after a couple of months.

Individuals can also take (simple) personal preventive measures such as reducing contacts to others or using hand sanitizer as discussed in Smith et al. [13]. Stoddard et al. [14] use a game-theoretic approach to explore noncompliance with non-pharmaceutical and biomedical measures during a pandemic and show that noncompliance can be an individually optimal choice in terms of cost-benefit under a broad set of conditions. Agusto et al. [15] study (rational) behavior of susceptible individuals in the context of social distancing to explain the second wave of the COVID-19 pandemic. Beyond previous results, Bughin et al. [16] highlight the trade-off between vaccination and non-pharmaceutical interventions such as social distancing as individuals balance these two types of prevention to protect against COVID-19.

Furthermore, Sabat et al. [17] find evidence for vaccination hesitancy as the most unstable vaccine intention with on average 42% of ever-hesitant individuals remaining hesitant in future waves. Understanding the dynamics and determinants of vaccination efforts is crucial for the success of targeted campaigns for future health crisis.

We contribute to a deeper understanding of vaccination hesitancy by examining factors associated with vaccination uptake, specifically differentiating between primary vaccination and booster uptake, as well as adherence to WHO recommendations on simple preventive behavior by investigating the role of Dark Triad (SD3) personality traits, i.e. Machiavellianism, narcissism, and psychopathy, as introduced by Paulhus and Williams [18], in these three prevention decisions.

The Dark Triad personality traits are relevant for identifying behaviors and power structures in groups [19], but also with COVID-19-related vaccination hesitancy [20] and for tailoring public health messages to increase adherence [21]. Individuals who score high in Machiavellianism are self-interested, ruthless, and cynical manipulators, who deceive for material gain, e.g. by misreporting [22]. Individuals with high narcissism have a sense of grandiosity and entitlement. High scores in psychopathy lead to antisocial behavior, impulsivity, and thrill-seeking. Psychopathy is associated with less compliant behavior as well as social distancing [23], and greater beliefs in COVID-19-related conspiracy theories [24]. However, other research suggests that a major health crisis itself can influence the stability of social preferences and behavior, as discussed by Shachat, Walker, and Wei [25], Li et al. [26] or Lohmann et al. [27]. While for example Lohmann et al. [27] find that high exposures to COVID-19 causes lead to an increase in anti-social behavior, mental health support in the early stages of a crisis may be critical.

We contribute to the existing literature on the demand for vaccines, Dark Triad personality traits, and behavior during the COVID-19 pandemic by specifically analyzing the propensity for booster shots and other simple preventive measures in addition to primary vaccination. These preventive behaviors are recommended by the WHO as socially preventive and responsible behavior [28]. We do find distinct associations between malevolent personality traits and these three different dimensions of prevention during the COVID-19 health crisis. Our analysis suggests that individuals with narcissistic personality traits are associated with a higher likelihood of reporting to refuse booster shots after having received the primary vaccinations, while individuals scoring high in psychopathy are significantly less likely to report to have received primary immunization. Individuals with higher psychopathic tendencies exhibit lower adherence to other simple preventive behavior, while higher scores in narcissism are associated with higher reported adherence.

We also contribute to the literature that investigates potential barriers and determinants of (general) vaccination uptake and prevention, ranging from socio-demographic factors, social norms and beliefs, and previous experiences [29,30,31], people’s level of religiosity [32, 33] or cognitive factors such as susceptibility and severity [34]. At the aggregate level, the literature emphasizes the role of the trustworthiness of public organizations or institutions [35,36,37], cost of vaccines [38], and anticipated consequences of non-pharmaceutical interventions [39]. Furthermore, Böhm and Betsch [40] and Böhm, Betsch, and Korn [41] find that pro-social concerns increase vaccination uptake. In addition, findings suggest that risk-averse individuals are more likely to be vaccinated as shown by Benzion, and Shahrabani [42], among others.

Our analysis is based on the longitudinal European COvid Survey (ECOS). The study was originally conducted to analyze people’s attitudes and concerns regarding the novel coronavirus SARS-CoV-2, including items from the COVID-19 Snapshot Monitoring Project (WHO). In particular, previous publications analyzed probable depression and anxiety during the pandemic [43], the uptake of the WHO recommendations [44], the link between altruism and pro-social pandemic behavior [45], people’s perceptions about the policy responses [46] or vaccination uptake in general [6].

After this introduction, the paper is structured as follows: We first introduce background of the Dark Triad personality and discuss potential effects on COVID-19-related preventive behavior. Methods section gives an overview of the data and methods we use in our analysis. We then show results from our analysis in Results section. Discussion section offers some discussion and policy implications are addressed. In Conclusions section, concluding thoughts are presented.

Dark Triad personality and COVID-19-related vaccination and other preventive behavior

The Dark Triad personality consists of the three conceptually distinct subclinical personality traits Machiavellianism, narcissism, and psychopathy [18]. While these constructs are of overlapping natureFootnote 1 the literature provides evidence of distinctive differences [18, 47,48,49]. With regard to the COVID-19 pandemic, some literature studies a potential differential impact of the three personality constructs on health-behavior endorsement and the appeal of public health messages [21] or psychological consequences [50, 51].

In the following, we discuss how the individual personality traits of the Dark Triad potentially affect primary immunization, booster uptake, and other simple preventive behavior. Table 1 summarizes the results of the discussion of the following three sections.

Table 1 Predictions of dark personality traits and COVID-19-related behavior

We do not expect individuals with high Machiavellian personality traits to differ from the rest of the sample with respect to the primary vaccination and booster shots, but to be more likely to adhere to preventive measures as we discuss below. We do not expect narcissistic individuals to be more likely primary immunized than the rest of the sample, but to be more likely to opt out from the booster shot. In addition, we expect individuals with high narcissistic personality traits to be more likely to report adherence to preventive measures as we explain in Narcissism section. For individuals with high psychopathic tendencies, we expect fewer vaccinations, less booster uptake, and less preventive behavior.

Machiavellianism

Machiavellians have the tendency to be manipulating, calculating, and ruthless individuals with little regard for others [18]. Generally, Machiavellians carefully consider the odds of their behavior and often use antisocial methods to achieve their goals, justified through rationalizations [52]. While these tendencies can lead to useful social outcomes under certain circumstances, Machiavellians can use the pandemic to exploit and manipulate others to serve their self-beneficial goals [48]. In sum, Machiavellianism comprises several different core themes and can be seen as a multidimensional construct [53].

As the primary immunizations and booster shots are considered safe and are recommended by health authorities, we would not expect Machiavellians to be more likely to opt out from vaccinations than individuals with less pronounced Machiavellian personality. This may hold true even though concerns about protecting others from infection can be considered a lower priority for Machiavellians. With respect to other simple preventive behavior, Machiavellians can be less affected by pandemic fatigueFootnote 2 due to their rationality and lower individual costs compared to vaccination and therefore adhere more consistently to preventive measures if they consider them effective. In addition, over-reporting of adherence can be seen as a reputation building tactic as Machiavellians may feel fewer moral concerns about not truthfully reporting own behavior [48].

Narcissism

Individuals with pronounced narcissistic personality traits are self-absorbed and egocentric and have a constant need for validation for their inflated self-view [18]. Furthermore, narcissists believe in their superiority and tend to be dismissive of advice from others [54, 55].

With respect to the primary immunization, narcissistic individuals consider their own health as more important than the health of other individuals and may feel entitled to be prioritized to get vaccinated. They consider themselves as good leaders and role-models for others [56]. The initially scarce and new technology of vaccines can serve as a potential source of narcissistic supply and self-validation. Seeking affirmation satisfies the grandiose self-concept of narcissists [57]. At the same time, individuals with high narcissistic personality traits may show aggravated concern about potential side effects and may not want to serve as “guinea pigs” on a newly developed vaccination technology, which can reduce the likelihood of getting vaccinated. Even though narcissistic individuals are more self-centered, being vaccinated and potentially protecting others from harm may be a way to gain social recognition for protecting others. Overall, these described motives and the self-serving behavior may all be present and we therefore do not come up with a clear prediction whether narcissistic individuals are more or less likely to have received primary immunization.

With respect to the booster uptake, concerns about protecting others from infection were less pronounced in the marketing campaign. Due to the declining protection of the primary immunization over time and the requirement to booster, the COVID-19 vaccination may be considered disappointing by some individuals. We hypothesize that feeling deceived about the short-term nature of protection from COVID-19 is potentially more pronounced for individuals with high narcissistic personality traits. Narcissistic personality has been shown to be associated with low levels of forgiveness, see Exline et al. [58]. Accordingly, narcissistic individuals can be expected to be less forgiving – also with respect to a vaccination, which did not provide as long-lasting coverage as expected. In addition, booster vaccinations were not scarce anymore at the time of the study. A generally available vaccine offers less narcissistic supply. Overall, the booster vaccination had less appeal to narcissistic individuals than the primary immunizations and we therefore expect that they are more likely to opt out from booster vaccinations after having received a primary immunization.

With respect to simple preventive behavior, narcissistic individuals may receive self-validation from displaying extremely careful behavior towards others. They may feel more shame to admit that they do not always adhere to meaningful preventive behavior and may therefore over-report their preventive behavior. Accordingly, we expect narcissistic individuals to report more preventive behavior.

Psychopathy

Pronounced psychopathic personality traits are associated with lack of empathy and remorse and bold, disinhibited behavior [18]. It is worth noting that psychopaths are more likely to act on temptation than Machiavellians and therefore show more impulsive behavior and more risk-taking [59]. Similarly, psychopathic individuals are also more pronounced to antisocial behavior and tend to aggression and physical threat [48, 59, 60].

Pro-social concerns of getting vaccinated and adhering to other simple preventive measures matter less to individuals with high psychopathic traits and their risk seeking tendencies can reduce the appeal of prevention. Accordingly, we predict that individuals with high psychopathic personality traits are less likely to get vaccinated, to receive a booster, and to engage in other preventive behavior.

In sum, as the three Dark Triad personality concepts show distinctive correlates we will investigate separate measurements in our analysis.

Methods

ECOS survey on COVID-19-related behavior

Our data utilizes wave nine from the European Covid Survey. This longitudinal study covers Germany, the United Kingdom, Denmark, Netherlands, France, Portugal, Italy, and Spain. In each country, data is collected from about 1,000 respondents. The initial wave of the web-based survey was conducted in April 2020. The respondents were recruited via the market research company Dynata. In order to ensure the representativeness with respect to gender, age, region, and education within each sample, country specific quotas based on the respective census data were used. We applied common quality controls, such as excluding individuals [61], who completed the questionnaire in less than a third of the median survey duration per country, as well as excluding multiple responses by the same person and incomplete responses. Respondents were paid by the panel provider for complete surveys, which is common practice in online data collections (see for example Smith et al. [13]). The questionnaire was programmed by the authors to avoid ordering effects by employing block randomizers and force response as a condition for the completion of the questionnaire to avoid missing data by design. A more detailed description of the ECOS survey and fieldwork can be found in Sabat et al. [62].

The data collection of wave nine started on 23 December 2021 and was completed on 10 January 2022. We restrict our analysis to Germany and the United Kingdom because the respective questionnaires include new questions on Dark Triad personality. Further details on the relevant parts of the questionnaire can be found in Appendix A.

As our analysis is based on data from Germany and the UK, we focus on the timeline of COVID-19 in these two countries. Even though the coronavirus hit Germany and the UK just days apart at the end of January 2020, both countries responded differently (see for example Van Kessel et al. [63] for an overview of different COVID-19 vaccination policies in Europe). At the time the data for this study was collected, booster shots were already available and all adults were eligible. Nevertheless, the booster campaign in the UK was further advanced than in Germany and the booster rate in the UK was about 10 percentage points higher as depicted in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1
figure 1

COVID-19 booster uptake in Germany and the UK. Total number of COVID-19 vaccine booster doses administered, divided by the total population of the country (in percentage). (Source: Official data collated by Our World in Data)

Measures

In our study, we investigate the association between dark personality traits and COVID-19-related preventive behavior, e.g. primary vaccination and booster uptake, and adherence to simple preventive measures. The respondent’s general COVID-19 vaccination acceptance (or hesitancy) is assessed with the actual vaccination status. The respondents indicate whether they have received no shot, the first shot or if they have already received primary immunization as they have received two shots or three shots (booster). The measure for simple preventive behavior comprises six items with four levels, covering the following aspects: regular hand washing; covering nose and mouth when sneezing; physical distancing; avoiding shaking hands, hugging, kissing; using alcohol-based hand rub; and avoiding touching the face. These measures reflect the initial recommendations by the WHO [28, 64] to reduce the spread of the pandemic and the individual risk of infection and became common measures of COVID-19 prevention in the literature (see for example Varghese et al. [44], Jordan, Yoeli, and Rand [65], Smith et al. [13], Wachira et al. [66]). Due to the panel nature of the ECOS data collection, the measures were repeatedly collected to allow comparisons over time. Similar to other COVID-19 related publications (see for example Smith et al. [13], Neumann-Böhme, Sabat, and Attema [45], Al-Zubaidy et al. [67]), we summarized the preventive behaviors in one index. The total adherence index was calculated by standardizing and adding up the scores of each item, divided by the number of items.

To explain COVID-19 vaccine-related decisions our variables of interest are malevolent personality traits, i.e. Machiavellianism, narcissism, and psychopathy. To assess Machiavellianism, narcissism, and psychopathy, the validated Short Dark Triad questionnaire (SD3) developed by Jones and Paulhus [49] is used. The Dark Triad score comprises nine items for each personality trait, totalling to 27 items. Answers can be given on a 5-point Likert-scale, ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree).Footnote 3 The Dark Triad score was derived using principal component analysis analogous to the approach from Pailing, Boon, and Egan [72], which is widely used in the literature. This score combines the three Dark Triad personality traits into one scalar by multiplying each mean response per personality trait with the respective factor loading and add them up to one index. Throughout the analysis, we use this measure as the Dark Triad score. In addition, we investigate the three Dark Triad personalities separately. For each personality score, we use the mean of the standardized answers to nine items.

In addition, we control for socio-economic factors that have been found to be associated with COVID-19-related decisions and are therefore included into our model [6, 43, 73]. Socio-economic controls include country of residence, age, gender, relative income (making ends meet), education, relationship status, number of children in the household, and risk groups present in the household (e.g. elderly persons or someone with diagnosed chronic medical conditions). In addition, we include the health risk attitude by 11 categories. To capture the individual health risk attitude, participants have to rate their willingness to take health risks on an 11-point Likert-scale from 0 (not at all willing to take health risks) to 10 (very willing to take health risks). To measure the health state we use the EQ-5D (see Herdman et al. [74]) and calculate a score for the health status. Well-being is measured using the ICECAP-A (see Al-Janabi, Flynn, and Coast [75]), where we again standardize the answers to calculate a score for the quality of life.

Analysis

We investigate the effect of the Dark Triad personality on vaccination uptake, e.g. to have received primary immunization and to already have received a booster shot (when having received the primary immunization) as well as adherence to simple preventive measures in response to COVID-19.

To estimate the effect of malevolent personality on vaccination uptake, our models are specified as follows:

$$\begin{aligned} Logit(Y_i) = \beta _0 + \beta _1 DarkTriad_i + \gamma X_{controls_i} + \epsilon _i\ \text {with}\ i=1,...,N \end{aligned}$$
(1)
$$\begin{aligned} Logit(Y_i) & = \beta _0 + \beta _1 psychopathy_i + \beta _2 narcissism_i + \beta _3 Machiavellianism_i \nonumber \\ & \quad + \gamma X_{controls_i} + \epsilon _i ~\text {with}~ i=1,...,N \end{aligned}$$
(2)

The dependent variable \(Y_i\) is the vaccination status, i.e. having received primary immunization and having received a booster shot (when having received primary immunization). The set of control variables described above is denoted by \(X_{controls_i}\). Socio-economic controls include country of residence, age, gender, income, education, the relationship status, a variable that indicates whether the household includes kids under the age of 18, a variable that indicates whether the household includes risk groups, self-reported health risk attitude, self-reported health status (EQ-5D), and self-reported quality of life (ICECAP-A). Our variable of interest in our first Logit estimation (1) is the Dark Triad score derived via principal component analysis.Footnote 4 In addition, in model (2) we investigate the three Dark Triad personality scores, i.e. on Machiavellianism, narcissism, and psychopathy, separately.

In addition, we investigate the link between Dark Triad personality and simple preventive behavior like physical distancing or regular hand washing. We estimate the following random effects models:

$$\begin{aligned} Preventive\ behavior_{ij} = \beta _0 + \beta _1 DarkTriad_{ij} + \gamma X_{controls_{ij}} + \alpha _j + \epsilon _{ij} ~\text {with}~ i=1,...,N \end{aligned}$$
(3)
$$\begin{aligned} Preventive~behavior_{ij} & = \beta _0 + \beta _1 psychopathy_{ij} + \beta _2 narcissism_{ij} + \beta _3 Machiavellianism_{ij} \nonumber \\ & \quad + \gamma X_{controls_{ij}} + \alpha _j + \epsilon _{ij} ~\text {with}~ i=1,...,N \end{aligned}$$
(4)

The dependent variable is the preventive behavior score. To assess preventive behavior, we use the mean of the standardized responses to the six items on preventive behavior. Random effects on country level are included. Our set of control variables \(X_{controls_{ij}}\) is the same as above. In addition, we include the vaccination status of the individual as an additional control. Again, in addition to investigating the Dark Triad score in model (3), we also account for each Dark Triad personality trait separately in model (4).

Results

Descriptive statistics

Our sample includes individual characteristics of 1,007 participants from Germany (DE) and 1,023 participants from the UK, totaling to 2,030 individuals. A small majority of the participants (52%) are female and 72% of the participants have no children. The mean age of the full sample is about 50 years. On average, 42% are able to make ends meet fairly easily, 44% have a high education level, and 49% are married.

In addition, our sample includes information on vaccination status, adherence to simple preventive measures, and Dark Triad personality. The majority of the sample have already received (at least) primary immunization as they have received two or three shots. On average, 86% (DE: 84%; UK: 87%) of the participants in our sample are primary vaccinated. In addition, most of the participants (whole sample: 61%; DE: 52%; UK: 70%) responded that they have already received a booster shot. Constraints on availability of and access to vaccination can be one explanation for the lower booster rate in Germany as the COVID-19 booster campaign was less evolved there compared to the UK. The mean (median) score of preventive behavior is 3.07 (3.17). With respect to the Dark Triad personality, individuals scored the highest on Machiavellianism. The overall Dark Triad score derived via principal component analysis ranges from −3.73 to 4.01. The Dark Triad personality scores do not differ substantially between Germany and the UK. Full tables on summary statistics are provided in Appendix B.

Dark Triad personality and primary immunization

In the following sections, we report our regression results. In our first model specification the dependent variable \(Y_i\) equals 1 if the individual has received primary immunization, as they have received two or three shots, and 0 if not. Therefore, our model estimates the likelihood of having received at least primary immunization. The control group are individuals who have received no shot or one shot. Table 2 shows estimation results from the logit model (1) and (2).

In column (1), the Dark Triad score is significant at the 5% level controlling for socio-economic factors. This implies that individuals scoring high at the SD3 are less likely primary immunized. An increase in the Dark Triad score of one unit is associated with a decrease in the likelihood of having received primary immunization of 1.29 percentage points. Our results in column (2) show that the negative correlation of SD3 and the likelihood of having received primary immunization is linked to psychopathy. The coefficient estimate is negative and significant at the 5% level, supporting the argument that individuals with higher psychopathic traits are less likely to get vaccinated. A one unit increase in the psychopathy score is associated with a decrease in the probability of having received primary immunization of 3.31 percentage points.

With regard to socio-economic controls, there is a positive relationship between age, income, the quality of life, and the likelihood of having received primary immunization. Individuals from the UK have significantly more likely received primary immunization which can be attributed to the earlier start of the vaccination campaign so that more people are already vaccinated at the timing of the survey. Being single and having kids has a significant negative correlation to vaccination acceptance. We report the full Table 2 including estimates on control variables in Appendix B.

Table 2 Dark Triad personality and primary immunization

Because of the timing of wave nine of the survey (23 December 2021 to 10 January 2022), we have to account for the variation in eligibility of and access to vaccines in Germany and the UK. Compared to Germany (84%), we observe a higher vaccination rate with regard to having received primary immunization in the UK (87%) as the vaccination campaign was further evolved in this country. In addition, while the majority of people in Germany have a clear preference for the Biontech/Pfizer vaccine (58%), people tend to prefer the AstraZeneca vaccine in the UK (18%) compared to Germany (2%) in our sample.

As a robustness check, we investigate the effect of Dark Triad personality on having received primary immunization for each country, i.e. Germany and UK, separately. Table 3 provides estimation results on our first model specification for Germany in column (1) and (2) and the UK in column (3) and (4), where we compare individuals who have already received (at least) primary immunization to those who have not. While we find no significant relationship for Dark Triad personalities in Germany, in the UK the negative correlation between psychopathy and primary immunization is even more pronounced and significant at the 5% level, which can be attributed to the different development of the vaccination campaign. Interestingly, we observe a positive association for Machiavellian personality and having received primary immunization in the UK, but the effect is only marginally significant.

Table 3 Dark Triad personality and primary immunization by country

Dark Triad personality and booster uptake

In a second model specification, the dependent variable \(Y_i\) equals 1 if the individual has already received a booster shot and 0 if not. The control group in this setting are individuals who have already received the primary vaccination. Therefore, our model estimates the likelihood of having received a booster shot when already having received primary immunization. Table 4 reports the regression results from logit model (1) and (2).

With respect to having already received a booster shot, the coefficient on Dark Triad is negative as well but not significant. In column (2), narcissism is significant at the 5% level, while psychopathy and Machiavellianism are not significant. Narcissistic individuals are associated with a higher likelihood of refusing booster shots after having received the primary vaccination. Specifically, a one unit increase in the score on narcissism is associated with a decrease in the probability of having received a booster shot (after having received primary immunization) of 4.52 percentage points. Neither do we find a significant correlation for Machiavellianism on the probability of already having received a booster shot nor for psychopathy.

Among utilized socio-economic controls, there is a positive relationship between age, education, income as well as the effect of risk groups in the household and the probability of being boostered (after having received primary immunization). Again, individuals from the UK are more likely boostered than individuals from Germany. Being single and having kids is significantly negatively correlated with booster uptake. We report the full Table 4 including estimates on control variables in Appendix B.

Table 4 Dark Triad personality and booster uptake

Again, because of the timing of wave nine of the survey, we have to account for the variation in eligibility of and access to booster in Germany and the UK. Compared to Germany, we observe a higher booster rate in the UK as the booster campaign was further evolved in this country. As a robustness check, we investigate the effect of Dark Triad personality on booster uptake for Germany and the UK, separately. Table 5 provides estimation results on our second model specification for Germany in column (1) and (2) and the UK in column (3) and (4). With regard to the relationship between Dark Triad personality and booster uptake in Germany, again we do not find any significant association controlling for socio-economic factors. If we only consider individuals from the UK in column (3) and (4), we find significant and even more pronounced effects of the Dark Triad personality on booster uptake compared to the results based on the full sample. Specifically, a one unit increase in the Dark Triad score is associated with a decrease in the probability of having received a booster shot (after having received primary immunization) of 1.95 percentage points. With regard to the three Dark Triad personality traits, again, we find a significant association for narcissism at the 5% level. A one unit increase in the score on narcissism is associated with a decrease in the probability of having received a booster shot of 5.92 percentage points. One reason for the more pronounced effect of narcissism on booster hesitancy in the UK can be due to the fact that more people preferred the AstraZeneca vaccine (18%) compared to Germany (2%). After the arising debate on the effectiveness of the AstraZeneca vaccine, people with narcissistic personality may even be more disappointed after having received primary immunization and are therefore more likely to opt out from the booster shot.

Table 5 Dark Triad personality and booster uptake by country

Dark Triad personality and simple preventive behavior

This section shows regression results where we investigate the link between Dark Triad personality and simple preventive behavior like physical distancing or regular hand washing. Table 6 reports regression results using a random effects model investigating the Dark Triad score in column (1) and each Dark Triad personality trait separately in column (2). The dependent variable is the preventive behavior score.

The coefficient estimates for psychopathy and narcissism are significant at the 1% level. Specifically, individuals scoring high at psychopathy are associated with significantly lower adherence to simple preventive behavior. In contrast to this finding, individuals with higher narcissistic personality are more likely to adhere to preventive behavior practices. Specifically, a one unit increase in the score of psychopathy is associated with a decrease in the score on preventive behavior of 0.211 points, while a one unit increase in the score on narcissism is associated with an increase in the adherence-score on preventive behavior of 0.115 points. This result seems to be robust throughout the six preventive recommendations, even if considerable heterogeneity can be observed with regard to adherence to each measure [44] (not shown in the table). With respect to socio-economic factors, being older, a higher reported health status and quality of life significantly correlates with an increase in adherence to preventive measures, while being male, a higher income, being single, and a higher self-reported willingness to take health risks is associated with lower adherence. The association for health status is only significant in column (1). Having received a booster shot (compared to having received primary immunization) is significantly associated with higher adherence to simple preventive measures, while not having received primary immunization (compared to having received primary immunization) correlates negatively with engagement into other preventive behavior measures. Again, we report the full Table 6 including estimates on control variables in Appendix B.

Table 6 Dark Triad personality and preventive behavior

To summarize, we find evidence that Dark Triad personality traits are significantly associated with primary vaccination and booster uptake as well as adherence to simple preventive measures. Therefore, our results highlight an important channel of vaccination hesitancy in addition to factors that have already been found to be associated with COVID-19-related behavior in previous studies. In addition, our results suggest to account for distinct aspects of the Dark Triad personality as psychopathy, narcissism, and Machiavellianism show different patterns of correlation with vaccination uptake and socially responsible behavior.

In addition, we conduct further sensitivity analyses in which we account for potential multicollinearity between the Dark Triad personality traits, restrict our sample, rerun the regression estimating a probit model, and use an alternative score as the Dark Triad score to ensure the robustness of our results presented in this chapter. Our findings show extremely robust estimates. Sensitivity analyses are shown in Appendix C.

Discussion

This paper investigated the relationship between the Dark Triad personality traits, vaccinations against COVID-19, and adherence to (simple) preventive behaviors. We find that dark personality traits correlate with the demand for vaccines, indicated by the vaccination status for both the primary and the booster vaccination as well as the adherence to other simple preventive measures in different ways. These findings suggest that tailored communication and delivery strategies may be beneficial in addressing potential psychological barriers to vaccination uptake.

The global COVID-19 pandemic has not only been a stress test for hospitals, outpatient services, and health administrations, but has also changed societies (see Vicente and Suleman [76] for an overview). For example, results from the US suggest that the pandemic highlighted the selfless behavior of others and a mutual dependence in society [77]. At the beginning of the pandemic, the public goods perspective of prevention was heavily emphasized: Individuals were asked to stay at home to protect elderly from harm, and vaccinations were also marketed as not only protecting the vaccinated person but also stopping the spread of the pandemic. Personality traits heavily influence our willingness to do things that benefit others. The literature on personality traits and COVID-19 behaviors suggests that individual risk perception and cognitive biases [78], personality differences [79], and individualism [80] play a role in adherence to social distancing and containment measures. Psychological characteristics such as conspiratorial and paranoid beliefs were related to vaccination hesitancy [20] and altruism plays a role in pro-social pandemic behaviors [45].

In this study, we find evidence that Dark Triad personality traits are associated with preventive behaviors with respect to the COVID-19 pandemic in several ways. These findings relate to earlier studies that for example link the DT personality traits, COVID-19-specific conspiracy beliefs and the willingness to be vaccinated in the future [24]. In the following, we discuss our results based on the predictions for each personality trait outlined in the Motivation section.

We expected that individuals with high scores in Machiavellian personality traits will be more likely to adhere to preventive measures and not differ from the rest of the sample regarding primary and booster vaccinations. As expected, we did not find a relationship between Machiavellianism and vaccination hesitancy but, interestingly, Konc, Petrović, and Dinić [81] showed that deviousness (an aspect of Machiavellianism) was associated with an unwillingness to vaccinate against COVID-19 independently of risk-taking tendencies. In contrast to the a priori expectations, we did not find a significant association between Machiavellianism and adherence to simple preventive behaviors.

Our prediction for narcissism was that respondents with narcissistic personality traits will not differ from the rest of the sample in terms of primary immunizations, but are more likely to opt out of booster shots in part because these were not scarce anymore and provide less narcissistic supply. We further predicted that respondents with higher narcissism scores were more likely to report adherence to preventive measures because they may receive self-validation from displaying extremely careful behavior towards others. As expected, we find no significant relationship between narcissism and primary vaccinations. In line with expectations, our findings show that narcissistic personality traits are associated with a higher likelihood of refusing booster shots after receiving the primary vaccinations. The differences are economically relevant, with marginal effects of 4.52 percentage points. In line with our findings, narcissism and psychopathy appear to be related to vaccination hesitancy and anti-vaccine outcomes [82]. Interestingly, Li and Cao [83] extend the dark personality traits and find that (everyday) sadism was also associated with more vaccine refusal. In line with our predictions, we find that higher scores of narcissism are positively associated with preventive behaviors.

Since psychopathy is associated with a lack of empathy and remorse as well as bold, disinhibited behavior, we expected respondents with high psychopathy scores to be unwilling to be vaccinated as well as less likely to engage in simple preventive behaviors. Contrary to our expectations, we did not find a significant relationship between psychopathy and booster vaccinations, while our other two predictions on psychopathy can be supported by the ECOS data. In line with the predictions, we find that individuals who scored higher in psychopathy were significantly less likely to have received primary immunization with marginal effects of 3.31 percentage points. One reason why we did not find a significant relationship for booster hesitancy is that psychopathic individuals may have already dropped out for primary immunization, as we only consider individuals that have at least received primary immunization to analyze booster uptake. Furthermore, individuals with higher psychopathic tendencies are associated with a lower engagement in simple preventive behaviors, which supports Konc, Petrović, and Dinić [81] findings, who show the same effect for recklessness as an aspect of psychopathy.

Overall, our results are almost completely in line with the theoretical predictions we made a priori about the three personality types and relate to earlier findings in the literature.

This study adds to the growing understanding of vaccination hesitancy and adherence to COVID-19 preventive measures. Our findings highlight the crucial role that personality traits, specifically the Dark Triad, play in these behaviors. The importance of addressing certain personality traits in (public) information campaigns to increase the effectiveness of the advertisement is well established in the marketing literature, for example in the domain of road safety advertising [84] or in the context of tailoring political advertisements towards voter’s personality traits [85]. In a health setting, Olsacher et al. [86] find a correlation between the big 5 personality traits and organ donation, with a higher effectiveness of messages that were tailored towards the personality traits. Related to COVID-19 and the dark triad personality traits, Huang, Yang, and Dai [87] find that people who score higher in Machiavellianism reported stronger anti-pandemic intentions (e.g. wanting to learn more about COVID-19) when a message was tailored towards the impact of COVID-19 on themselves compared to messages highlighting the impact towards others. Policymakers, health professionals, and those in charge of public health messaging may take these factors into account when devising communication strategies to improve the vaccination uptake and adherence to preventive behaviors.

Firstly, to address narcissistic tendencies, messaging around booster vaccines should be tailored to appeal to individuals’ sense of uniqueness and importance. Strategies could include emphasizing the advanced technology used in the booster shots, making it a ’premium’ choice. This approach could also leverage on exclusivity or status to appeal to the inherent desire for admiration that characterizes this personality trait.

Secondly, our findings show that individuals with psychopathic tendencies are associated with a lower likelihood of getting vaccinated and adhering to preventive measures. Given the spontaneous and thrill-seeking nature associated with high psychopathy levels, alternative approaches could be explored to target this group. One could consider spontaneous vaccination offers in environments that appeal to their impulsivity, such as casinos or theme parks. Quick-access, walk-in vaccination centers, or pop-up clinics in highly trafficked areas might also provide the spontaneity that would appeal to such individuals.

Lastly, there is a need for greater public education and transparent communication to tackle the influence of conspiracy theories, which is particularly relevant for individuals with high dark triad personality traits due to the greater susceptibility to conspiracy theories. Policymakers should prioritize evidence-based, accessible information to debunk common myths and conspiracies surrounding COVID-19 vaccinations.

Some limitations need emphasizing: First, we used self-reported vaccination status from a representative online survey and are not able to verify the official status. Respondents with high scores in the Dark Triad personality traits could be more likely to misrepresent their answers. At the same time, the anonymity and online nature of the study provided no incentives to provide socially desirable answers and we find a substantial difference in vaccination decisions based on the Dark Triad personality traits psychopathy and narcissism. Furthermore, employing online panels to elicit the populations preferences was a common and useful way to attain data in the pandemic. Nonetheless, it came with certain drawbacks, for example problems to reach older parts of the population and the risk of people filling out these surveys professionally. We carefully designed the online questionnaire to avoid that respondents were unable to complete the questionnaire, i.e. because of a lack of understanding of questions or, on the other hand, that some respondents speed through the survey to be rewarded without providing meaningful information. We therefore had to make certain design decisions such as the use of the force response option to avoid missing data. Since we provided middle options such as “neither agree nor disagree”, we believe respondents had options to express their preferences to continue. Second, as indicated in figure 1, the vaccination campaigns in the UK and Germany differed concerning the availability of vaccines due to the earlier Conditional Marketing Authorization of vaccines in the UK, which may have resulted in higher reported vaccination rates in the UK in this study. We find a higher association of Dark Triad personality traits with booster vaccinations, which can be due to the differences in roll-out. Lastly, we caution the reader to interpret our results as correlations rather than causal effects as the observed behavior itself may influence or even reinforce personality traits. While existing literature predominantly suggests Dark Triad personalities to be rather stable or slightly decreasing over longer time horizons (see for example Edelstein, Newton, and Stewart [88], Lynam et al. [89], Grosz et al. [90]), we cannot fully rule out that COVID-19 preventive measures may influence self-reporting of personality traits. While our findings indicate statistically significant associations, we acknowledge that other unobserved factors may contribute to these relationships. Future longitudinal or experimental studies could provide deeper insights into the directional effects of personality traits on health-related decision-making.

Conclusions

Our results suggest that personality types should be considered for such information campaigns to frame messages and adopt policies that appeal for example to narcissists, because even small changes in messaging have shown to affect the level of adherence of different personality types [91]. These findings provide insights for future outbreaks and pandemics to achieve higher vaccination rates and more adherence to protective measures. Lastly, the relevance of psychopathic personality traits and the association with vaccine related conspiracy theories [24] may explain some of the radical intensity in the public and private debates about the COVID-19 vaccination.

In conclusion, there is a necessity of a targeted, nuanced approach to public health messaging to appeal to parts of the population that score high in the Dark Triad personality traits. While broad, universal messaging serves a purpose, acknowledging the impact of personality traits on health behaviors has the potential to reach and convince groups who might otherwise stay unwilling to be vaccinated. Future pandemics and public health crises would benefit from this understanding to promote greater public adherence to protective measures and lower levels of vaccination hesitancy.

Data availability

No datasets were generated or analysed during the current study.

Notes

  1. We observe intercorrelations of about 40% to 55% in our data. The highest correlation between narcissism and psychopathy is consistent with the original study from Paulhus and Williams [18].

  2. The term pandemic fatigue is defined by WHO as the unwillingness to comply with recommended protective behavior for expanded time periods.

  3. The SD3 questionnaire has been translated into German for use with German-speaking individuals. The German version of the SD3 serves as a reliable proxy for German samples, demonstrating high cross-language replicability, as evidenced by several studies (see for example Malesza et al. [68], Wehner et al. [69]) and was also assessed in other (cross-cultural) studies [70, 71].

  4. However, our results are the same if we use the mean of all personality responses for each individual as the Dark Triad score instead.

References

  1. Dubé E, Gagnon D, Nickels E, Jeram S, Schuster M. Mapping vaccine hesitancy - Country-specific characteristics of a global phenomenon. Vaccine. 2014;32(49):6649–54.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Kata A. Anti-vaccine activists, Web 2.0, and the postmodern paradigm-An overview of tactics and tropes used online by the anti-vaccination movement. Vaccine. 2012;30(25):3778–89.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Mak DB, Daly AM, Armstrong PK, Effler PV. Pandemic (H1N1) 2009 influenza vaccination coverage in Western Australia. Med J Aust. 2010;193(7):401–4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Cubizolles C, Barjat T, Chauleur C, Bruel S, Botelho-Nevers E, Gagneux-Brunon A. Evaluation of intentions to get vaccinated against influenza, COVID 19, pertussis and to get a future vaccine against respiratory syncytial virus in pregnant women. Vaccine. 2023;41(49):7342–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Courbage C, Peter R. On the effect of uncertainty on personal vaccination decisions. Health Econ. 2021;30(11):2937–42.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Neumann-Böhme S, Varghese NE, Sabat I, Barros PP, Brouwer W, van Exel J, et al. Once we have it, will we use it? A European survey on willingness to be vaccinated against COVID-19. Eur J Health Econ. 2020;21(7):977–82.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Patwary MM, Bardhan M, Disha AS, Hasan M, Haque MZ, Sultana R, et al. Determinants of COVID-19 vaccine acceptance among the adult population of Bangladesh using the health belief model and the theory of planned behavior model. Vaccines. 2021;9(12):1393.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Campos-Mercade P, Meier AN, Schneider FH, Meier S, Pope D, Wengström E. Monetary incentives increase COVID-19 vaccinations. Science. 2021;374(6569):879–82.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Müller S, Rau HA. Economic preferences and compliance in the social stress test of the COVID-19 crisis. J Public Econ. 2021;194:104322.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Zettler I, Schild C, Lilleholt L, Kroencke L, Utesch T, Moshagen M, et al. The role of personality in COVID-19-related perceptions, evaluations, and behaviors: Findings across five samples, nine traits, and 17 criteria. Soc Psychol Personal Sci. 2022;13(1):299–310.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Ścigała KA, Schild C, Moshagen M, Lilleholt L, Zettler I, Stückler A, et al. Aversive personality and COVID-19: A first review and meta-analysis. Eur Psychol. 2021;26(4):348.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Polack FP, Thomas SJ, Kitchin N, Absalon J, Gurtman A, Lockhart S, et al. Safety and efficacy of the BNT162b2 mRNA Covid-19 vaccine. N Engl J Med. 2020;383(27):2603–15.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Smith LE, Potts HW, Amlôt R, Fear NT, Michie S, Rubin GJ. Engagement with protective behaviours in the UK during the COVID-19 pandemic: a series of cross-sectional surveys (the COVID-19 rapid survey of adherence to interventions and responses \(\left[CORSAIR\right]\) study). BMC Public Health. 2022;22(1):1–11.

  14. Stoddard M, Van Egeren D, Johnson KE, Rao S, Furgeson J, White DE, et al. Individually optimal choices can be collectively disastrous in COVID-19 disease control. BMC Public Health. 2021;21(1):1–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Agusto FB, Erovenko IV, Fulk A, Abu-Saymeh Q, Romero-Alvarez D, Ponce J, et al. To isolate or not to isolate: The impact of changing behavior on COVID-19 transmission. BMC Public Health. 2022;22(1):1–20.

    Google Scholar 

  16. Bughin J, Cincera M, Kiepfer E, Reykowska D, Philippi F, Żyszkiewicz M, et al. Vaccination or NPI? A conjoint analysis of German citizens’ preferences in the context of the COVID-19 pandemic. Eur J Health Econ. 2023;24(1):39–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Sabat I, Neumann-Böhme S, Barros PP, Torbica A, van Exel J, Brouwer W, et al. Vaccine hesitancy comes in waves: Longitudinal evidence on willingness to vaccinate against COVID-19 from seven European countries. Vaccine. 2023;41(36):5304–12.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Paulhus DL, Williams KM. The dark triad of personality: Narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy. J Res Pers. 2002;36(6):556–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Makowsky MD, Smaldino PE. The evolution of power and the divergence of cooperative norms. J Econ Behav Organ. 2016;126:75–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Murphy J, Vallières F, Bentall RP, Shevlin M, McBride O, Hartman TK, et al. Psychological characteristics associated with COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy and resistance in Ireland and the United Kingdom. Nat Commun. 2021;12(1):1–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Blagov PS. Adaptive and dark personality in the COVID-19 pandemic: Predicting health-behavior endorsement and the appeal of public-health messages. Soc Psychol Personal Sci. 2021;12(5):697–707.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Lohse T, Simon SA, Konrad KA. Deception under time pressure: Conscious decision or a problem of awareness? J Econ Behav Organ. 2018;146:31–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Triberti S, Durosini I, Pravettoni G. Social distancing is the right thing to do: Dark Triad behavioral correlates in the COVID-19 quarantine. Personal Individ Differ. 2021;170:110453.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Hughes S, Machan L. It’s a conspiracy: Covid-19 conspiracies link to psychopathy, Machiavellianism and collective narcissism. Personality Individ Differ. 2021;171:110559.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Shachat J, Walker MJ, Wei L. How the onset of the Covid-19 pandemic impacted pro-social behaviour and individual preferences: Experimental evidence from China. J Econ Behav Organ. 2021;190:480–94.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Li KK, Hong YY, Huang B, Tam T. Social preferences before and after the onset of the COVID-19 pandemic in China. Judgment Decis Mak. 2022;17(6):1313–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Lohmann PM, Gsottbauer E, You J, Kontoleon A. Anti-social behaviour and economic decision-making: Panel experimental evidence in the wake of COVID-19. J Econ Behav Organ. 2023;206:136–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. World Health Organization. Critical Preparedness, Readiness and Response Actions for COVID 19–Interim Guidance. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2020

  29. Schmid P, Rauber D, Betsch C, Lidolt G, Denker ML. Barriers of influenza vaccination intention and behavior-a systematic review of influenza vaccine hesitancy, 2005–2016. PLoS ONE. 2017;12(1):e0170550.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Wu YY, Zhang W. Demographic disparities in COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy among US adults: Analysis of household pulse survey data from Jul 21 to Oct 11 in 2021. Vaccine. 2022;40(52):7510–4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Willis DE, Moore R, Andersen JA, Li J, Selig JP, McKinnon JC, et al. Correlates of COVID-19 vaccine coverage in Arkansas: Results from a weighted random sample survey. Vaccine. 2023;41(41):6120–6.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Stillman S, Tonin M. Communities and Testing for COVID-19. Eur J Health Econ. 2022;23(4):617–25.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Lahav E, Shahrabani S, Rosenboim M, Tsutsui Y. Is stronger religious faith associated with a greater willingness to take the COVID-19 vaccine? Evidence from Israel and Japan. Eur J Health Econ. 2021;1–17.

  34. An S, Schulz PJ, Kang H. Perceived COVID-19 susceptibility and preventive behaviors: moderating effects of social support in Italy and South Korea. BMC Public Health. 2023;23(1):1–11.

    Google Scholar 

  35. Dubé E, Laberge C, Guay M, Bramadat P, Roy R, Bettinger JA. Vaccine hesitancy: an overview. Hum Vaccines Immunotherapeutics. 2013;9(8):1763–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Reinhart AM, Tian Y, Lilly AE. The role of trust in COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy and acceptance among Black and White Americans. Vaccine. 2022;40(50):7247–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Choi Y, Park S, Lee J, Kim Y, Kim BJ, Lin L, et al. Who gets COVID-19 booster vaccination? Trust in public health institutions and promotion strategies post-pandemic in the Republic of Korea. Vaccine. 2023;41(50):7560–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. MacDonald NE. Vaccine hesitancy: Definition, scope and determinants. Vaccine. 2015;33(34):4161–4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Mühlbacher AC, Sadler A, Jordan Y. Population preferences for non-pharmaceutical interventions to control the SARS-CoV-2 pandemic: trade-offs among public health, individual rights, and economics. Eur J Health Econ. 2022;23(9):1483–96.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Böhm R, Betsch C. Prosocial vaccination. Curr Opin Psychol. 2022;43:307–11.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Böhm R, Betsch C, Korn L. Selfish-rational non-vaccination: Experimental evidence from an interactive vaccination game. J Econ Behav Organ. 2016;131(B):183–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Tsutsui Y, Benzion U, Shahrabani S. Economic and behavioral factors in an individual’s decision to take the influenza vaccination in Japan. J Socio-Econ. 2012;41(5):594–602.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Hajek A, Sabat I, Neumann-Böhme S, Schreyögg J, Barros PP, Stargardt T, et al. Prevalence and determinants of probable depression and anxiety during the COVID-19 pandemic in seven countries: Longitudinal evidence from the European COvid Survey (ECOS). J Affect Disord. 2022;299:517–24.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Varghese NE, Sabat I, Neumann-Böhme S, Schreyögg J, Stargardt T, Torbica A, et al. Risk communication during COVID-19: A descriptive study on familiarity with, adherence to and trust in the WHO preventive measures. PLoS ONE. 2021;16(4):e0250872.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Neumann-Böhme S, Sabat I, Attema AE. Altruism and the Link to Pro-social Pandemic Behavior. Front Health Serv. 2022;2:871891.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Sabat I, Neumann-Böhme S, Varghese NE, Barros PP, Brouwer W, van Exel J, et al. United but divided: Policy responses and people’s perceptions in the EU during the COVID-19 outbreak. Health Policy. 2020;124(9):909–18.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Vernon PA, Villani VC, Vickers LC, Harris JA. A behavioral genetic investigation of the Dark Triad and the Big 5. Personal Individ Differ. 2008;44(2):445–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Horowitz LM, Strack S (Eds.). Handbook of interpersonal psychology: Theory, research, assessment, and therapeutic interventions. New York; Wiley. (pp. 249 –268).

  49. Jones DN, Paulhus DL. Introducing the short dark triad (SD3) a brief measure of dark personality traits. Assessment. 2014;21(1):28–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Modersitzki N, Phan LV, Kuper N, Rauthmann JF. Who is impacted? Personality predicts individual differences in psychological consequences of the COVID-19 pandemic in Germany. Soc Psychol Personal Sci. 2021;12(6):1110–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Hardin BS, Smith CV, Jordan LN. Is the COVID-19 pandemic even darker for some? Examining dark personality and affective, cognitive, and behavioral responses to the COVID-19 pandemic. Personal Individ Differ. 2021;171:110504.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Zeigler-Hill V , Marcus DK (Eds.). The dark side of personality. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

  53. Rauthmann JF, Will T. Proposing a multidimensional Machiavellianism conceptualization. Soc Behav Personal Int J. 2011;39(3):391–403.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Kausel EE, Culbertson SS, Leiva PI, Slaughter JE, Jackson AT. Too arrogant for their own good? Why and when narcissists dismiss advice. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process. 2015;131:33–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. O’Reilly CA, Hall N. Grandiose narcissists and decision making: Impulsive, overconfident, and skeptical of experts-but seldom in doubt. Personal Individ Differ. 2021;168:110280.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Furtner MR, Rauthmann JF, Sachse P. The self-loving self-leader: an examination of the relationship between self-leadership and the dark triad. Soc Behav Personal Int J. 2011;39(3):369–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Morf CC, Rhodewalt F. Unraveling the paradoxes of narcissism: A dynamic self-regulatory processing model. Psychol Inq. 2001;12(4):177–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Exline JJ, Baumeister RF, Bushman BJ, Campbell WK, Finkel EJ. Too proud to let go: narcissistic entitlement as a barrier to forgiveness. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2004;87(6):894.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Williams KM, Nathanson C, Paulhus DL. Identifying and profiling scholastic cheaters: Their personality, cognitive ability, and motivation. J Exp Psychol Appl. 2010;16(3):293.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Baughman HM, Dearing S, Giammarco E, Vernon PA. Relationships between bullying behaviours and the Dark Triad: A study with adults. Personality Individ Differ. 2012;52(5):571–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Zhang C, Conrad F. Speeding in web surveys: The tendency to answer very fast and its association with straightlining. In: Survey research methods. vol. 8. Southampton: European Survey Research Association (ESRA), University of Southampton, Department of Social Statistics and Demography. 2014. pp. 127–35.

  62. Sabat I, Neumann-Böhme S, Stargardt T, Schreyögg J. The European COvid Survey (ECOS): Technical Report. HCHE Research Paper. 2024.

  63. Van Kessel R, Forman R, Milstein R, Mastylak A, Czabanowska K, Czypionka T, et al. Divergent COVID-19 vaccine policies: policy mapping of ten European countries. Vaccine. 2023;41(17):2804–10.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  64. World Health Organization. Infection prevention and control during health care when novel coronavirus (nCoV) infection is suspected: interim guidance, 25 January 2020. Switzerland: World Health Organization; 2020.

  65. Jordan JJ, Yoeli E, Rand DG. Don’t get it or don’t spread it: Comparing self-interested versus prosocial motivations for COVID-19 prevention behaviors. Sci Rep. 2021;11(1):20222.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  66. Wachira E, Laki K, Chavan B, Aidoo-Frimpong G, Kingori C. Factors influencing COVID-19 prevention behaviors. J Prev. 2023;44(1):35–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  67. Al-Zubaidy N, Fernandez Crespo R, Jones S, Drikvandi R, Gould L, Leis M, et al. Exploring the relationship between government stringency and preventative social behaviours during the COVID-19 pandemic in the United Kingdom. Health Inform J. 2023;29(4):14604582231215868.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Malesza M, Ostaszewski P, Büchner S, Kaczmarek MC. The adaptation of the Short Dark Triad personality measure-psychometric properties of a German sample. Curr Psychol. 2019;38:855–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Wehner C, Maaß U, Leckelt M, Back MD, Ziegler M. Validation of the Short Dark Triad in a German sample: Structure, nomological network, and an ultrashort version. Eur J Psychol Assess. 2021;37(5):397.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  70. Aluja A, Garcia LF, Rossier J, Ostendorf F, Glicksohn J, Oumar B, et al. Dark triad traits, social position, and personality: a cross-cultural study. J Cross-Cult Psychol. 2022;53(3–4):380–402.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. Jaeger T, Steinorth P. Investment Gambling and Dark Triad Personality Traits (July 11, 2022). Available at SSRN: https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.4184399.

  72. Pailing A, Boon J, Egan V. Personality, the Dark Triad and violence. Personal Individ Differ. 2014;67:81–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  73. Andersson O, Campos-Mercade P, Meier AN, Wengström E. Anticipation of COVID-19 vaccines reduces willingness to socially distance. J Health Econ. 2021;80:102530.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  74. Herdman M, Gudex C, Lloyd A, Janssen M, Kind P, Parkin D, et al. Development and preliminary testing of the new five-level version of EQ-5D (EQ-5D-5L). Qual Life Res. 2011;20:1727–36.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  75. Al-Janabi H, Flynn TN, Coast J. Development of a self-report measure of capability wellbeing for adults: the ICECAP-A. Qual Life Res. 2012;21:167–76.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Vicente P, Suleman A. COVID-19 in Europe: from outbreak to vaccination. BMC Public Health. 2022;22(1):1–17.

    Google Scholar 

  77. Cappelen AW, Falch R, Sørensen EØ, Tungodden B. Solidarity and fairness in times of crisis. J Econ Behav Organ. 2021;186:1–11.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  78. Nikolov P, Pape A, Tonguc O, Williams C. Predictors of Social Distancing and Mask-Wearing Behavior: Panel Survey in Seven U.S. States. IZA Discussion Paper No. 13745. Available at SSRN: https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3704126.

  79. Carvalho LDF, Pianowski G, Gonçalves AP. Personality differences and COVID-19: are extroversion and conscientiousness personality traits associated with engagement with containment measures? Trends Psychiatry Psychother. 2020;42:179–84.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Chen C, Frey CB, Presidente G. Culture and contagion: Individualism and compliance with COVID-19 policy. J Econ Behav Organ. 2021;190:191–200.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  81. Konc I, Petrović K, Dinić BM. Dark Tetrad and COVID-19 protective measures: Mediating effects of risk-taking tendencies. Personal Individ Differ. 2022;186:111341.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  82. Howard MC. The good, the bad, and the neutral: Vaccine hesitancy mediates the relations of Psychological Capital, the Dark Triad, and the Big Five with vaccination willingness and behaviors. Personal Individ Differ. 2022;190:111523.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  83. Li H, Cao Y. Your pain, my gain: The relationship between self-report and behavioral measures of everyday sadism and COVID-19 vaccination intention. Curr Psychol. 2023;42(23):20149–56.

  84. D’Souza C, Tay R. Advertising implications and design of messages. Mark Intell Plan. 2016;34(4):504–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  85. Zarouali B, Dobber T, De Pauw G, de Vreese C. Using a personality-profiling algorithm to investigate political microtargeting: assessing the persuasion effects of personality-tailored ads on social media. Commun Res. 2022;49(8):1066–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  86. Olsacher A, Bade C, Ehlers J, Fehring L. How to effectively communicate health information on social media depending on the audience’s personality traits: An experimental study in the context of organ donation in Germany. Soc Sci Med. 2023;335:116226.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  87. Huang Y, Yang S, Dai J. Self-versus other-directed outcomes, Machiavellianism, and hypothetical distance in COVID-19 antipandemic messages. Soc Behav Personal Int J. 2021;49(3):1–13.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  88. Edelstein RS, Newton NJ, Stewart AJ. Narcissism in midlife: Longitudinal changes in and correlates of women’s narcissistic personality traits. J Personal. 2012;80(5):1179–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  89. Lynam DR, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Loeber R, Stouthamer-Loeber M. Longitudinal evidence that psychopathy scores in early adolescence predict adult psychopathy. J Abnorm Psychol. 2007;116(1):155.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  90. Grosz MP, Göllner R, Rose N, Spengler M, Trautwein U, Rauthmann JF, et al. The development of narcissistic admiration and machiavellianism in early adulthood. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2019;116(3):467.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Blais J, Chen PG, Pruysers S. Who complies and who defies? Personality and public health compliance. Front Polit Sci. 2021;3:660911.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank our colleagues in the ECOS consortium for their cooperation in this project. We would also like to thank our translators Asgar Bo Frederiksen, Julien Bergeot, Joana Pestana, Maarten Husen, and Elisabetta Oliveri who translated the survey into their native languages. We acknowledge financial support from the Open Access Publication Fund of Universität Hamburg.

Funding

Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL. This project was supported by the Horizon 2020 Research and Innovation Programme of the European Union through the Marie Skłodowska-Curie Grant Agreement No. 721402 and was further funded by the German Research Foundation (DFG) under grant number 466310982. It also received funds from the Excellence Strategy of the University of Hamburg and other participating universities.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

SNB conceptualized and implemented the survey. SB and PS developed the methodology of the study, conducted the analysis and data visualizations and wrote the original draft. SNB provided revisions and validation. All authors read and approved the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sophia Bock.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Ethics approval was obtained by the University of Hamburg Social Science (WiSo) Research Laboratory for the complete ECOS study in April 2020. There were no privacy concerns since no personal or identifiable information was recorded. The study is in compliance with the terms of use of the WiSo Laboratory, the guidelines for safeguarding good scientific practice and avoiding scientific misconduct at the University of Hamburg as well as the RESPECT code of practice released by the RESPECT project by order of the European Commission. All authors of the study declared to follow and uphold these scientific and ethical standards.

All participants provided their informed consent before starting the survey.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Bock, S., Neumann-Böhme, S. & Steinorth, P. Impact of Dark Triad personality traits on COVID-19 vaccination uptake and prevention efforts: insights from the European Covid Survey (ECOS). BMC Public Health 25, 1352 (2025). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-025-22471-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-025-22471-3

Keywords