- Research
- Open access
- Published:
Impact of food availability and predator presence on patterns of landscape partitioning among neighbouring Guinea baboon (Papio papio) parties
Movement Ecology volume 13, Article number: 9 (2025)
Abstract
Background
Access to critical resources, including food, water, or shelter, significantly determines individual fitness. As these resources are limited in most habitats, animals may employ strategies of landscape partitioning to mitigate the impact of direct resource competition. Territoriality may be regarded as an aggressive form of landscape partitioning, but other forms of landscape partitioning exist in non-territorial species. Animals living in groups with greater flexibility in their association patterns, such as multilevel societies with fission–fusion dynamics, may adjust their grouping and space use patterns to short-term variations in ecological conditions such as food availability, predation pressure, or the presence of conspecific groups. This flexibility may allow them to balance the costs of competition while reaping the benefits of better predator detection and defence.
Methods
We explored patterns of landscape partitioning among neighbouring Guinea baboon (Papio papio) parties in the Niokolo-Koba National Park, Senegal. Guinea baboons live in a multilevel society in which parties predictably form higher-level associations (“gangs”). We used four years of locational data from individuals equipped with GPS collars to estimate annual home ranges, home range overlap, and average minimum distances between parties. We examined whether food availability and predator presence levels affected the cohesion between parties in 2022.
Results
We found substantial overlap in home range and core area among parties (33 to 100%). Food availability or predator presence did not affect the distance to the closest neighbouring party; the average minimum distance between parties was less than 100 m.
Conclusions
Our results suggest a low level of feeding competition between our study parties. Whether this is a general feature of Guinea baboons or particular to the situation in the Niokolo-Koba National Park remains to be investigated.
Background
Access to critical resources such as food, water, shelter, or mates can be a significant determinant of the fitness of animals. In habitats where specific resources are limited, there is a high potential for resource competition [1]. An extensive body of theoretical and empirical research targets whether and how animals, particularly primates, adjust aspects of their social organisation, including group size, composition and cohesion, to prevailing ecological conditions in their home ranges [2,3,4,5,6,7,8,9,10]. These studies have highlighted the importance of resource abundance and distribution in shaping primates' spatial distribution and social organisation. Specifically, more abundant and evenly distributed resources are expected to result in larger, more cohesive social groups [2, 5, 11]. Many primate species have been shown to adjust their group size to the distribution and size of feeding patches with smaller feeding parties when feeding patches are small and spatially or temporally clumped, likely resulting in reduced costs of intragroup feeding competition [e.g., 12, 13]. While competition for resources presumably sets an upper limit to group size and cohesion in group-living animals, predation pressure is often considered to promote the formation of larger groups. Larger groups can benefit from enhanced predator detection, risk dilution, communal defence, or mobbing of predators [3].
In habitats where ecological conditions are not stable but vary temporally (e.g., seasonally), fission–fusion dynamics can serve as a strategy to balance the costs and benefits of group living [6]. Animals that exhibit fission–fusion dynamics are thought to flexibly adjust their group size, cohesion and composition to different degrees in response to varying ecological conditions [5, 6, 14, 15]. One way such fission–fusion dynamics can manifest is by partitioning a shared landscape.
Animals may exhibit purely spatial partitioning, where certain parts of the landscape areas are used exclusively. If these areas are defended, this leads to territoriality [16]. In non-territorial species, animals may avoid regions or parts of their home ranges recently used by neighbouring groups or sub-groups in species with fission–fusion dynamics, thus preventing associated costs of aggressive encounters [16,17,18,19,20]. Another way to mitigate resource competition with neighbouring groups that inhabit the same space is to share the same area but temporally avoid other groups or subgroups. Such temporal avoidance could greatly reduce the potential for aggressive encounters and contest competition for valuable resources [19,20,21].
In this study, we investigated patterns of landscape partitioning among neighbouring Guinea baboon (Papio papio) parties in the vicinity of Simenti, Niokolo-Koba National Park, Senegal. Guinea baboons are group-living, non-territorial primates that are highly spatially tolerant, with most inter-group encounters being neutral or even affiliative [22, 23]. They form large multi-male, multi-female groups of 20 to more than 300 individuals. Guinea baboons have a nested multi-level social organisation with fission–fusion dynamics [22]. At the basis of the multi-level society is the one-male unit, consisting of one primary male, one to several females, their dependent offspring, and sometimes secondary males [24]. Several one-male units form parties, which, together with other parties, form gangs [25]. Over 90% of the offspring in a unit are sired by the primary male of a unit at the time of conception, significantly reducing the potential for mate competition [26]. Events of fissions and fusions in Guinea baboons usually happen along consistent lines and most often at the level of parties. Parties can vary in size but frequently consist of more than 20 individuals, including juveniles. The home ranges of parties in the Niokolo-Koba National Park in Senegal vary between 20 and 50 km2 [23, 27]. Like other baboon species, Guinea baboons are considered eclectic omnivores feeding primarily on fruit but also on other plant parts, insects, and occasionally on small birds or mammals [23, 27]. In our study area, the most important predators of Guinea baboons are African lions (Panthera leo), leopards (P. pardus), and spotted hyenas (Crocuta crocuta) [28].
We aimed to identify whether and how neighbouring Guinea baboon parties partition a shared landscape intraspecifically, both purely spatially and temporally, and to assess the extent to which their space use reflects avoidance or attraction-based patterns. We hypothesised that the degree of spatial and temporal overlap between parties varies based on the level of resource competition and predation pressure. We expected avoidance-based patterns to become more pronounced when resource (food) availability was low, likely related to increased levels of feeding competition and potentially irrespective of predator presence. This would result in reduced spatial overlap between the core areas of different parties or increased temporal avoidance of neighbouring parties to reduce direct encounters while occupying the same overall area. Conversely, we expected to see more attraction-based patterns when predator presence was high, as closer cohesion could provide more effective protection from predators. This might result in pronounced spatial overlap between core areas or spatial proximity between parties, suggesting that the benefits of group living related to predator presence or the lack of significant competition outweigh the potential costs.
Methods
Study site
The data collection for this study took place at the long-term field site of the Centre de Recherche de Primatologie (CRP), Simenti (13°01 ‘34 ‘‘ N, 13°17 ‘41 ‘‘ W) in the Niokolo-Koba National Park in south-east Senegal (Fig. 1). The study site is part of the Sudanian and Sahelo-Sudanian climatic zone with pronounced seasonality and high seasonal variability in rainfall [29]. The average annual precipitation in Simenti is around 950 mm. The rainy season lasts from June to October, with May and mid-October constituting transitional periods with minor and variable rainfall [30]. The vegetation represents a mosaic of grasslands, wooded savannahs, and gallery forests along streams and other perennial water bodies [29, 30]. Several habituated Guinea baboon parties live at the study site, and researchers have followed them extensively since 2007.
Data collection
GPS Data
To assess the spatial distribution of Guinea baboon parties across the study area, we equipped eight adult males, each representing the location of their entire party (P5, P6I, P6W, P7, P9B, P13, P15, P17), with GPS collars (Tellus 2 Basic Light) with integrated drop-off mechanisms from January to December 2022. Party membership was assigned based on spatial proximity, and individuals spent most of their time with members of their parties [22]. The GPS collars were programmed to take locational fixes every two hours during the day (06:00 to 18:00) and an additional three fixes during the night to mark the location of the sleeping site (21:00, 00:00, 03:00). We downloaded GPS data from the collars using a UHF antenna monthly.
In addition to the data from 2022, locational data from a previous study were available for 2010 to 2012 [22]. This dataset comprised locational data with identical sampling intervals for eleven individuals (four females) from six different parties in 2010, seven individuals (two females) from six parties in 2011 and five individuals (one female) from five parties in 2012. As this dataset also contained individuals from unhabituated parties, we excluded these individuals from all analyses to remove any effects of habituation as a potential confounding factor.
Capture and collaring
Collaring for this study took place on five days between January 16 and January 21, 2022. We located the targeted baboons and their parties in the morning, followed them and short-term immobilised them covertly (without either the target animals themselves or surrounding individuals seeing the shot) while they were ranging with their parties by darting via a blowpipe (all darting equipment was from TeleDart: blowpipe B16, calibre 16 mm, length 160 cm, 2 ml darts (BD2) with plastic stabilisers (BST16) and plain needles 1.5 × 38 mm [TDN1538LL]). The initial dose for chemical immobilisation per baboon was 100 mg ketamine, 20 mg xylazine, and 2 mg atropine. As soon as the target baboon was fully immobilised and all other baboons had left the area (at least 300 m from the immobilised target baboon), we moved the baboon to a well-shaded area, blindfolded the male and started to monitor vital parameters (oxygen saturation, heart rate, and internal body temperature). We weighed the animal using a spring scale. The mean body mass of males was 21.5 kg ± 1.9 SD (range: 19–25 kg, n = 8). After collaring, we moved the animal close to a small tree in the shade and administered 1.5 mg of atipamezole as an antidote against xylazine. We closely surveyed the baboon from a distance until it was fully awake and able to move safely. All collared baboons successfully rejoined their respective parties on the same or the following day. A veterinarian of the Senegalese Direction des Parcs Nationaux (DPN) and a representative of the veterinary service of Tambacounda accompanied and supervised the entire capture and collaring procedure. For the collaring procedures from 2010 to 2012, see Knauf et al. [31].
Phenological data
To estimate food availability across the home range of our study parties, we recorded the phenological state of known feeding tree species from November 2021 to March 2023. To this end, we selected 31 feeding tree species that our study parties fed on regularly during a previous study [23] (Table S1). For each of these feeding tree species, we selected ten individual trees on average (range 1–13) that, if possible, were distributed evenly across the different habitat types our study population occupies. The trees were marked with aluminium tags and visited at the beginning of each month. We recorded each tree’s phenological state according to its phenological activity (eight levels: none, young leaves, flower buds, flowers, young fruits, intermediate fruits, ripe fruits, and mature leaves). We considered trees bearing ripe or intermediate fruit as “providing food”. As our study population also consumed the flowers of certain tree species, we included months in which those trees were flowering as “providing food”. For each tree species, we calculated food availability as the proportion of trees providing food (either fruiting or flowering) relative to the total number of trees monitored for that species. Finally, we calculated the monthly food availability score by averaging these proportions across all observed tree species.
Predator presence
To estimate the predator presence at our study site, we used non-baited motion-triggered camera traps distributed across 37 km2, covering most of our study parties’ home ranges. The cameras were deployed on a grid of 1 × 1 km, with roughly one camera per km2 (Fig. 2).
All cameras except one were installed facing towards frequently used animal trails. We installed cameras in February 2022 and retrieved them in March 2023. In the meantime, we exchanged batteries and SD cards monthly. During the rainy season, we cleared an area of ca. 5 m around each camera from grass to ensure unobstructed visibility and avoid excessive triggering of the cameras by vegetation. The cameras were programmed to take three consecutive pictures upon being triggered, with a 1-min interval between triggers. The imagery obtained from this camera-trapping grid was annotated using the online platform Agouti [32]. The AI-assisted annotation was manually checked for all images [33]. We recorded animal species and the number of individuals for all images taken by the camera traps. We considered all sightings of lions, leopards, or hyenas within the camera-trapping grid as a predator encounter. In addition to data from the camera trapping grid, we used ad libitum data on all signs of predators (tracks, scat, calls, sightings) recorded at the field site during the study period. From both camera trapping data and ad libitum observational data, we counted the number of predator encounters in the study area at 14-day intervals. As we could not be sure about the most relevant time window for assessing perceived predation pressure in Guinea baboons, we also evaluated predator encounters over shorter (2 and 7 days) and longer (30 days) intervals.
Statistical analyses
Spatial landscape partitioning
We estimated each party's annual home ranges and core areas using the package amt [34] in R version 4.3.1 [35]. We delineated home ranges and core areas as the 95% and 50% contour levels obtained from Kernel Density Estimation (KDE), respectively. We used a rule-based ad hoc approach (SCALEDh) to select bandwidths within a search range of 0.01 and 1 of REFh [36]. For 2010–2012, we calculated home ranges and core areas for all individuals that belonged to one party collectively, as several individuals per party were collared. Further, as there was substantial overlap between collaring periods in 2011 and 2012, we pooled locational data for individuals from the same parties for overlapping periods. We then used the function ‘hr_overlap’ from the package amt [34] to calculate home range overlap between parties per year.
Temporal landscape partitioning
To assess temporal patterns of cohesion between our study parties, we calculated the average distance between pairs of collared individuals, each representing their entire party’s location, for each day when locational data were available for both parties. Then, we identified the shortest distance between all dyads of parties until each party was represented at least once for every day it was observed on the same day as another party. This procedure allowed us to determine the average minimum distance between neighbouring parties each day (Fig. 3).
Exemplary depiction of the assessment of average minimum distances between parties. For the exemplary day, GPS data were available for five parties (P5, P6W, P9B, P13, P17). We averaged the distances between parties across the day. We then identified the closest neighbouring dyads (P5 and P13, P9B and P17, P6W and P17) and the Euclidean distances between them (d1-d3)
We used minimum rather than average distances between parties because, in a limited space, movements away from one party would often lead to proximity to another, leading to averages not being able to capture the spatial relationships we were interested in. By using average minimum distances between parties rather than individual locational points collected throughout the day, we aimed to mitigate the potential temporal autocorrelation in the data. Temporal autocorrelation arises because consecutive locational waypoints (e.g., taken two hours apart) are most likely more similar to one another than waypoints taken further apart in time. Parties observed in close proximity in the morning are more likely to remain close to each other for considerable periods of time (clearly extending beyond a sampling interval of two hours) due to spatial constraints limiting their movement within this sampling interval. Using daily averages allowed us to capture general association patterns while minimising the bias introduced by temporal dependencies.
We also used this approach to validate our assumption that one individual of a party is representative of the entire party’s location. To this end, we compared average minimum distances between individuals from the same party to average minimum distances between members of different parties from 2010 to 2012 (Fig. S1).
We further assessed the number of neighbouring parties within close spatial proximity on a given day. Therefore, we defined a distance threshold using GPS collar data from individuals of the same party collared in 2010. Specifically, we used the 95th percentile of these distances as our cutoff, which captures the range of distances over which individuals from the same party typically spread (429 m). This threshold enabled us to identify how many collared parties in 2022 were close enough to likely move together as one functional “social entity”. We counted the number of neighbouring parties within this distance threshold for each day and party. We then visually inspected the relationship between the number of parties in proximity, food availability, and predator presence throughout the year. We did not model these data because of non-independence in them. For instance, the same party might be within the distance threshold of two or more parties simultaneously. Whenever two parties, A and B, were within the threshold distance, we counted A within the threshold distance of B and B within the threshold distance of A.
Modelling
To assess whether patterns of temporal landscape partitioning varied with food availability and predator presence, we used data on average minimum distances between parties, food availability, and predator presence only from 2022. We fitted a multiple membership model using the function brm of the package brms version 2.20.4 [37] in R version 4.3.1 [35]. We log-transformed the average minimum distance between parties to increase the probability of model convergence. We included log distance as the response variable, the food availability score, and the number of predator encounters within 14 days before the observation as predictors. We included the IDs of the two individuals of a dyad (as a multi-membership term) and also dyad ID as random intercepts effect and random slopes of food availability and predator presence within 14 days in the model to avoid overconfident model estimates [38, 39]. A multi-membership term means that only one random intercepts effect is estimated for both individuals involved in a dyad. This accounts for the possibility that some parties are generally closer to others than other parties. The dyad effect in term accounts for the possibility that some parties tend to associate with one another more than others. We fitted a multi-membership model since the two individuals of a dyad could not be unambiguously assigned to two different random effects variables. We checked for sufficient variation in food availability and predator presence within each individual before fitting the model. We also included parameters for the correlations among random intercepts and random slopes in the model. As we received a warning about divergent transitions during warm-up with the default settings of brm, we set adapt_delta to 0.9.
We fitted three additional models, including the number of predators per 2, 7 and 30 days instead of 14 days as predictors, but with an otherwise identical structure to ensure model results were not biased by an inappropriate choice of the time window used to assess predator presence. To check whether the resulting patterns were solely driven by Guinea baboon parties aggregating at sleeping sites in the mornings or evenings, we fitted four additional models with identical structures, using only the minimum distance between parties at noon as the response.
Results
Across all study years, we obtained 58,404 locational fixes during the day, excluding those taken during the night (21:00, 00:00, 03:00). In 2022, 17,365 of these locational fixes were collected during 347 unique days. The GPS collars lasted 316 days on average, with one collar (for P6I) failing after 162 days due to water damage.
Spatial patterns of landscape partitioning
In 2022, our study parties consisted of 25 individuals on average (including males, females, and juveniles), ranging from seven individuals in P15 to 41 individuals in P5. We collectively estimated annual home ranges and core areas for parties P4, P5, P6, and P9 for 2010, as well as for 2011 to 2012. For 2022, we estimated the home ranges and core areas of parties P5, P6I, P6W, P7, P9B, P13, P15, and P17. The average home range size in 2022 was 38.4 km2 (range 20.6–45.9 km2), and core areas were around 8.5 km2 (range 3.5–10.9 km2). The average overlap between home ranges of parties in 2022 was 89% (range 45–100%, Table 1). Core area overlap between parties in 2022 was 80% on average (range 33–98%, Table 2).
Food availability
Food availability scores ranged from 0.03 to 0.33 across the study period (Fig. 4A). The highest scores were recorded in February, March, and April 2022. In contrast, the lowest scores were recorded in June, July, and August 2022.
Predator presence
In 2022, we registered 588 predator encounters, 376 of which were ad libitum records and 212 were camera trap images of predators. Most records were from spotted hyenas (254), followed by lions (211) and leopards (118). Of the remaining five records, one was a sighting of a pack of eleven African wild dogs (Lycaon pictus), and four records could not be reliably identified as either leopard or lion.
The average number of predator encounters in the 14-day interval was 25 (median, range 17 – 42). The average number of predator encounters in the 7-day interval was 13 (median, range 7 – 27). The average number of predator encounters in the 30-day interval was 53 (median, range 39 – 82), and for the 2-day interval, it was around 3 (median, range 2–13).
Temporal patterns of landscape partitioning
The average minimum distance between neighbouring Guinea baboon parties in 2022 was 81.4 m (median, 47.1 – 207.3 m IQR) (Fig. 5). Throughout the year, we observed short average minimum distances between neighbouring parties (Fig. 6). There were many instances of close proximity and fewer instances of larger distances between parties. However, these average minimum distances were notably larger in April and May, increasing from approximately 50 m to 100 m. Visual inspection of the number of parties in close proximity relative to varying levels of food availability and predator presence showed no clear relationship between these factors (Fig. 7).
Number of parties in close spatial proximity at varying levels of food availability A and the number of predator encounters per two weeks B in 2022. The distance threshold of 429 m was established using the 95th percentile of locational data of individuals of the same party collared in 2010. The area of the dots depicts the frequency with which a given number of parties in proximity per day of observation occurred per monthly value of food availability (range 1 – 93) (A) and per number of predator encounters per two weeks (range 1 – 67) B
We did not find evidence for our study parties to adjust their space use patterns alongside varying levels of food availability (Fig. 8A) or predator presence (Fig. 8B; Table 3). The estimated effect sizes were marginal, regardless of the time interval chosen when calculating predator presence (Tables S6-S8). Model results did not reveal any effect of predator presence and food availability when considering only waypoints taken at noon (Tables S9-S12).
Model results for the effect of food availability A and the number of predator encounters per two weeks B on the average minimum distance between parties in 2022. For A, predator presence was centred to a mean of zero, and for B, food availability was centred to a mean of zero. The average minimum distance between parties (y-axis) is depicted on a log scale for visual clarity. The fitted mean is shown as a dashed line, and confidence intervals are shaded in grey
Discussion
Spatial landscape partitioning
Our analyses revealed substantial overlap between home ranges and core areas of most of the observed Guinea baboon parties. Almost all parties’ home ranges overlapped by 90%, and core areas overlapped by 80% on average, except for one party (P7). Although most individuals were habituated to the presence of researchers in the field, this party was not part of our regular study parties but probably belonged to a different gang than the other study parties. While the home ranges of the other study parties overlapped relatively little with that of P7, P7's home range was considerably smaller and completely enclosed within the home ranges of the other parties.
As troops in chacma baboons (P. chacma) and bands in hamadryas baboons (P. hamadryas) correspond more closely to the level of gangs rather than parties in Guinea baboons [22, 42], our results of home range overlap cannot be compared to other studies directly. However, our results also indicate considerable overlap (45–100%, depending on the perspective of comparison) between neighbouring gangs. The amount of home range overlap between Guinea baboon parties of the same and different gangs in Simenti was considerably higher than reported for chacma baboon troops in Suikerbosrand Nature Reserve in South Africa. The average overlap in South Africa was ca. 5% (median, range 0 – 53.72%) during the wet season and ca. 3% (median, range 0 – 45.34%) during the dry season [40] In Erer-Gota, Ethiopia, home ranges of bands of hamadryas baboons overlapped by about 50% with at least seven neighbouring bands [41]. Similar to what we observed in Guinea baboons in Simenti, Altmann & Altmann [43] and Markham et al. [19] found a pronounced overlap between home ranges of neighbouring troops of yellow baboons (P. cynocephalus) in Amboseli, Kenya. The authors concluded that the baboons most likely occupied no part of their study troops’ home ranges exclusively. Yellow baboons did, however, spend less time in overlapping areas and had fewer encounters with neighbouring groups than would be expected by chance, suggesting an avoidance-based pattern of landscape partitioning [19].
Similarly, owl monkeys (Aotus azarae) in Northern Argentina showed relatively pronounced overlap between home ranges (48% ± 15%) but minimal overlap between core areas of 11% on average, likely to maintain exclusive access to clumped resources in their core areas [44]. A study on space partitioning in mountain gorillas (Gorilla beringei beringei) found home range overlap between neighbouring groups of 42% on average (median, range 9.7—94.8%) but substantially lower overlap between core areas, with an average of under 10% for 7 out of 10 study groups. The authors concluded that this might be a strategy to maintain exclusive access to vital resources located in the core areas for a non-territorial species [20].
Contrary to our initial assumption, the Guinea baboons did not seem to maintain exclusive access to essential resources in the core areas. Instead, they exhibited similarly high levels of overlap in these areas, as seen across their entire home range. We, therefore, conclude that Guinea baboon parties at our field site do not show a purely spatial pattern of landscape partitioning. This may be due to the high population size of Guinea baboons in the Niokolo-Koba National Park. Population estimates in 1998 and 2018 suggested a population size between 100,000 and 250,000 individuals [45, 46], translating to population densities of 10.9 individuals per km2 or 27.4 individuals per km2, respectively (but see also Sharman [27] with 8.7 baboons/km2). The population of Guinea baboons in the Niokolo-Koba National Park may thus already be close to carrying capacity, and all suitable habitat for Guinea baboons is likely occupied, so it may be impossible to avoid neighbouring parties purely spatially.
Temporal landscape partitioning
When looking at the temporal patterns of cohesion between parties, we found that most parties stayed in close spatial proximity to at least one other party, with an average minimum distance of less than 100 m. The exception was again party P7, which seemed further away from the other study parties on average. P7 is not one of our regular study parties and belongs to a different gang; unsurprisingly, we found greater distances between them and the other parties. Given that we equipped only eight parties that inhabit the study area with GPS collars, it is very likely that P7 also stayed near other non-collared parties. Although we observed an increase in average minimum distances between parties from around 50 m to approximately 100 m in April and May, this change likely has no substantial biological implications related to predator detection and defence or feeding competition between parties. This increase may reflect a more extensive group spread while parties remained nearby or mingled with other parties.
Our model results revealed no effect of food availability or predator presence on the cohesion between parties, at least at the spatiotemporal resolution we used to assess food availability and predator presence. Despite substantial variation in both ecological variables across the study period, Guinea baboons stayed in very close spatial proximity regardless of food availability or predator presence. This observation did not support our hypothesis that Guinea baboons would flexibly adjust their proximity to neighbouring parties to reduce feeding competition when food availability was lower or to increase the potential for predator detection and defence when predator presence was higher.
Other studies on fission–fusion societies have produced mixed results regarding how ecological conditions affect party or subgroup cohesion within and among species. For instance, spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi) exhibited varying association patterns affected by seasons, food availability, and precipitation. However, those effects were highly context-dependent, varying with the sex of the study animals, their habitats, and whether association or proximity patterns were considered [47, 48]. Similarly, party sizes of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) have been linked to food availability [12, 49], predation pressure [50] and the number of receptive females within the party [51, 52] in some studies but not others [53].
While these species exhibit more individualistic patterns of fission–fusion dynamics, with variable party membership that changes frequently, species with more predictable subgroup composition also show mixed results. Yunnan snub-nosed monkeys (Rhinopithecus bieti) displayed an increase in fission events and subgrouping when important seasonal components of their diet were available, but not in relation to predator presence [54]. Another study on the same species found no effect of food availability on subgrouping [55]. In hamadryas baboons, bands fissioned more frequently during warmer and wetter months, coinciding with periods during which preferred food items such as doum palm fruits (Hyphaene spp.) were less abundant – likely as a strategy to mitigate feeding competition during these months [13, 56, 57].
One reason for the lack of a statistically significant effect of predator presence on the temporal cohesion between parties may have been the consistently high levels of predator presence throughout the study period so that the perceived risk for the baboons did not vary, even when we, according to our proxies, determined a lower predator presence. Although our dataset showed considerable variation in predator presence (Fig. 4B), there were no periods entirely free of or particularly low in predator encounters. For instance, within any 2-day interval, there were at least two predator encounters. Such a constantly high predator presence may have prevented Guinea baboon parties from adjusting their spatial cohesion even if predator presence temporarily decreased slightly. Additionally, our measures of predator presence may have been too coarse to reliably capture changes in the perceived risk that might affect the grouping and space use of our study parties.
Our relatively coarse assessment of food availability may also affect our findings. We monitored the phenology of a proportion of known feeding tree species at a monthly interval. However, we did not monitor all known feeding tree species or herbaceous plant foods consumed by our study parties, nor did we assess feeding tree distribution across the landscape. We could also not determine the abundance and distribution of other food types, such as invertebrates readily consumed by our study parties.
Interestingly, in 2022, the period of lowest food availability according to our selection of feeding tree species was during the rainy season in June, July, and August. Many grass shoots and herbaceous plants became available during this time, and our study parties fed on them. Additionally, this period coincided with a mass occurrence of caterpillars of a species of hawk moths (Sphingidae) (Fig. 9), which made up 65% of our study parties’ feeding time in July. For comparison, in other months, insects made up only 4% of our study parties’ feeding time on average.
Therefore, our study parties could likely tolerate prolonged periods of low plant food availability. The lack of a statistically significant effect of plant food availability, as we determined it, on the cohesion between parties may result from very low levels of feeding competition between parties in the study area so that even when fewer feeding trees are bearing fruit, alternative food sources are sufficiently available. The benefits of group living (e.g., support in conflicts, protection against predators) outweigh the costs related to feeding competition in our study population in Simenti, Senegal, thus allowing parties to stay in close spatial proximity year-round. However, it is possible that resource partitioning and competition are less relevant at the landscape level but become more pronounced at a finer scale [58, 59], such as within feeding patches or at the level of units rather than parties. At this more granular level, competition for highly valuable, contestable resources may be more immediate and intense, potentially leading to larger spacing between individuals foraging together.
Conclusion
We found no evidence for spatial or temporal avoidance between neighbouring Guinea baboon parties. On the contrary, we found strong evidence for most of the observed parties for attraction-based space use patterns. Fission–fusion dynamics at the party level in our study period did not appear as a strategy to mitigate resource competition or the risks associated with predator presence, at least not based on our analytical approach. Most study parties stayed in close spatial proximity to at least one other party, irrespective of variation in these ecological variables. Our findings indicate no avoidance-based patterns related to food availability, suggesting low levels of resource competition between parties when assessed across their entire home ranges. Nevertheless, resource competition may be more pronounced within specific feeding patches among individuals foraging together. At this finer scale, competition might manifest as increased spacing between foraging individuals or higher aggression over valuable, contestable resources.
Data availability
GPS data from 2010 to 2012 can be found on Göttingen Research Online: https://doi.org/10.25625/IHEZUE [23]. GPS and phenological data, as well as data on predator presence from 2022 are available from the corresponding author or from JF upon request.
References
Morales JM, Moorcroft PR, Matthiopoulos J, Frair JL, Kie JG, Powell RA, et al. Building the bridge between animal movement and population dynamics. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2010. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0082.
Crook JH, Gartlan JS. Evolution of primate societies. Nature. 1966. https://doi.org/10.1038/2101200a0.
van Schaik CP. Why are diurnal primates living in groups? Behaviour. 1983. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853983X00290.
Wrangham RW. An ecological model of female-bonded primate groups. Behaviour. 1980. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853980X00447.
Janson CH. Primate socio-ecology: The end of a golden age. Evol Anthropol. 2000. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1520-6505(2000)9:2%3c73::AID-EVAN2%3e3.0.CO;2-X.
Aureli F, Schaffner CM, Boesch C, Bearder SK, Call J, Chapman CA, et al. Fission-fusion dynamics: New research frameworks. Curr Anthropol. 2008. https://doi.org/10.1086/586708.
Lawler RR.Feeding competition, cooperation, and the causes of primate sociality: a commentary on Sussman, et al. Am J Primatol. 2011. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20816.
Sussman RW, Garber PA, Cheverud JM. Reply to Lawler: feeding competition, cooperation, and the causes of primate sociality. Am J Primatol. 2011. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20815.
Clutton-Brock T, Janson CH. Primate socioecology at the crossroads: Past, present, and future. Evol Anthropol. 2012. https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.21316.
Koenig A, Scarry CJ, Wheeler BC, Borries C. Variation in grouping patterns, mating systems and social structure: what socio-ecological models attempt to explain. Phil Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2013. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2012.0348.
Janson CH, Goldsmith ML. Predicting group size in primates: foraging costs and predation risks. Behav Ecol. 1995. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/6.3.326.
Chapman CA, Wrangham RW, Chapman LJ. Ecological constraints on group size: an analysis of spider monkey and chimpanzee subgroups. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1995. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00175729.
Schreier AL, Swedell L. Ecology and sociality in a multilevel society: Ecological determinants of spatial cohesion in hamadryas baboons. Am J Phys Anthropol. 2012. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.22080.
Henzi SP, Lycett JE, Piper SE. Fission and troop size in a mountain baboon population. Anim Behav. 1997. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1996.0302.
Pike KN, Perry J. Love thy neighbour: Feral buffalos show greater space use, resource overlap and encounters during the wet season in the Northern Territory. Ecol Evol. 2024;14(3): e70345. https://doi.org/10.1002/ece3.70345.
Burt WH. Territoriality and home range concepts as applied to mammals. J Mammal. 1943. https://doi.org/10.2307/1374834.
Brown JL. The evolution of diversity in avian territorial systems. Wilson Bull. 1964;76:160–9.
Brown JL, Orians GH. Spacing patterns in mobile animals. Annu Rev Ecol Evol Syst. 1970. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.es.01.110170.001323.
Markham AC, Guttal V, Alberts SC, Altmann J. When good neighbors don’t need fences: temporal landscape partitioning among baboon social groups. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2013. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-013-1503-9.
Seiler N, Boesch C, Mundry R, Stephens C, Robbins MM. Space partitioning in wild, non-territorial mountain gorillas: the impact of food and neighbours. R Soc Open Sci. 2017. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.170720.
Engel A, Reuben Y, Kolesnikov I, Churilov D, Nathan R, Genin A. Space partitioning within groups of social coral reef fish. Coral Reefs. 2024;43(3):1–12. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00338-023-02460-x.
Patzelt A, Kopp GH, Ndao I, Kalbitzer U, Zinner D, Fischer J. Male tolerance and male–male bonds in a multilevel primate society. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2014. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1405811111.
Zinner D, Klapproth M, Schell A, Ohrndorf L, Chala D, Ganzhorn JU, et al. Comparative ecology of Guinea baboons (Papio papio). 2021. Primate Biol. https://doi.org/10.5194/pb-8-7-2021.
Goffe AS, Zinner D, Fischer J. Sex and friendship in a multilevel society: behavioural patterns and associations between female and male Guinea baboons. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2016. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-015-2050-6.
Fischer J, Kopp GH, Dal Pesco F, Goffe A, Hammerschmidt K, Kalbitzer U, et al. Charting the neglected West: The social system of Guinea baboons. Am J Phys Anthropol. 2017. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.23144.
Dal Pesco F, Trede F, Zinner D, Fischer J. Male–male social bonding, coalitionary support and reproductive success in wild Guinea baboons. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 2022. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2021.2189.
Sharman MJ. Feeding, ranging and social organisation of the Guinea baboon [PhD Thesis]. University of St Andrews; 1982.
Horion R, Woodgate Z, Drouilly M. First insights into the spatio-temporal ecology of sympatric large carnivores in Niokolo-Koba National Park, Senegal. Oryx. 2024;1–12. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0030605323001746.
Arbonnier M. Arbres, arbustes et lianes des zones sèches d’Afrique de l’Ouest, vol. 2. Montpellier: CIRAD, MNHN; 2002.
Burgess N, Hales J, Underwood E, Dinerstein E, Olson D, Itoua I, et al. Terrestrial eco-regions of Africa and Madagascar: A conservation assessment. Washington: Island Press; 2004.
Knauf S, Barnett U, Maciej P, Klapproth M, Ndao I, Frischmann S, et al. High prevalence of antibodies against the bacterium Treponema pallidum in Senegalese Guinea baboons (Papio papio). PLoS ONE. 2015. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0143100.
Casaer J, Milotic T, Liefting Y, Desmet P, Jansen P. Agouti: A platform for processing and archiving of camera trap images. Biodivers Inf Sci Stand. 2019. https://doi.org/10.3897/biss.3.37637.
Brouillet, A. Spatio-temporal pattern of space use by predators and prey: A camera-trap-based approach in a West African savannah-woodland habitat [MSc thesis]. Georg-August-University Göttingen. 2024.
Signer J, Fieberg J, Avgar T. Animal movement tools (amt): R package for managing tracking data and conducting habitat selection analyses. Ecol Evol. 2019. https://doi.org/10.1002/ece3.4823.
R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2023.
Kie JG. A rule-based ad hoc method for selecting a bandwidth in kernel home-range analyses. Anim Biotelemetry. 2013. https://doi.org/10.1186/2050-3385-1-13.
Bürkner PC. brms: An R Package for Bayesian multilevel models using Stan. J Stat Softw. 2017. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v080.i01.
Barr D, Levy R, Scheepers C, Tily H. Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. J Mem Lang. 2013. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001.
Schielzeth H, Forstmeier W. Conclusions beyond support: overconfident estimates in mixed models. Behav Ecol. 2009. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arn145.
Slater K, Barrett A, Brown LR. Home range utilization by chacma baboon (Papio ursinus) troops on Suikerbosrand Nature Reserve. South Africa PLoS One. 2018. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0194717.
Sigg H, Stolba A. Home range and daily march in a hamadryas baboon troop. Folia Primatol. 1981. https://doi.org/10.1159/000156008.
Kummer H. The social system of hamadryas baboons and its presumable evolution. In: de Mello MT, Whiten A, Bzrne RW, editors. Baboons: Behaviour and ecology, Use and care. Brasilia, Brazil: Selected Proceedings XIIth Congress of the International Primatological Society; 1990. p. 43–60.
Altmann SA, Altmann J. Baboon ecology. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1970.
Wartmann FM, Juárez CP, Fernandez-Duque E. Size, site fidelity, and overlap of home ranges and core areas in the socially monogamous owl monkey (Aotus azarae) of Northern Argentina. Int J Primatol. 2014. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-014-9771-7.
Galat-Luong A, Galat G, Hagell S. The social and ecological flexibility of Guinea baboons: implications for Guinea baboon social organization and male strategies. In: Swedell L, Leigh SR, editors. Reproduction and fitness in baboons: Behavioral, ecological, and life history perspectives. New York: Springer; 2006. p. 105–21.
Rabeil T, Hejcmanová P, Gueye M, Greffrath R, Cornut D. Inventaire combiné terrestre et aérien, Parc National du Niokolo-Koba, Sénégal. Niokolo Koba National Park, Senegal; 2018.
Aguilar-Melo AR, Calmé S, Smith-Aguilar SE, Ramos-Fernandez G. Fission-fusion dynamics as a temporally and spatially flexible behavioral strategy in spider monkeys. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2018. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-018-2562-y.
Aguilar-Melo AR, Calmé S, Pinacho-Guendulain B, Smith-Aguilar SE, Ramos-Fernández G. Ecological and social determinants of association and proximity patterns in the fission–fusion society of spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi). Am J Primatol. 2020. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.23077.
Anderson DP, Nordheim EV, Boesch C, Moermond TC. Factors influencing fission–fusion grouping in chimpanzees in the Taï National Park, Côte d’Ivoire. In: Boesch C, Hohmann G, editors. Behavioural Diversity in Chimpanzees and Bonobos. 1st ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2002. p. 90–101.
Boesch C. The effects of leopard predation on grouping patterns in forest chimpanzees. Behav. 1991. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853991X00544.
Mitani JC, Watts DP, Lwanga JS. Ecological and social correlates of chimpanzee party size and composition. In: Boesch C, Hohmann G, Marchant L, editors. Behavioural Diversity in Chimpanzees and Bonobos. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2002. p. 102–11.
Matthews JK, Ridley A, Kaplin BA, Grueter CC. Ecological and reproductive drivers of fission-fusion dynamics in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) inhabiting a montane forest. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2021. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-020-02964-4.
Lehmann J, Boesch C. To fission or to fusion: effects of community size on wild chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes verus) social organisation. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2004. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-004-0781-x.
Ren B, Li D, Garber PA, Li M. Fission–fusion behavior in Yunnan snub-nosed monkeys (Rhinopithecus bieti) in Yunnan. China Int J Primatol. 2012. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-012-9586-3.
Bai SC, Zou SQ, Lin S, Tuo D, Tu Z, Zhong T. A preliminary observation on distribution, number and population structure of Rhinopithecus bieti in the Baima Xueshan Natural Reserve, Yunnan. China Zool Res. 1987;8:413–9.
Henriquez MC, Amann A, Zimmerman D, Sanchez C, Murray S, McCann C, et al. Home range, sleeping site use, and band fissioning in hamadryas baboons: Improved estimates using GPS collars. Am J Primatol. 2021. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.23248.
Schreier AL. Feeding ecology, food availability and ranging patterns of wild hamadryas baboons at Filoha. Folia Primatol. 2010. https://doi.org/10.1159/000316562.
Webber QMR, Albery GF, Farine DR, Pinter-Wollman N, Sharma N, Spiegel O, Vander Wal E, Manlove K. Behavioural ecology at the spatial–social interface. Biol Rev. 2023;98(3):868–86. https://doi.org/10.1111/brv.12934.
Picardi S, Abrahms BL, Merkle JA. Scale at the interface of spatial and social ecology. Philos Trans R Soc B. 1912;2024(379):20220523.
Acknowledgements
We are grateful to the Direction des Parcs Nationaux (DPN) and the Ministère de l’Environnement et de la Protéction de la Nature (MEPN) de la République du Sénégal for the approval to conduct this study in the Niokolo-Koba National Park. We especially thank the former conservateur of the park, Jacques Gomis, for his support. We are grateful to all the CRP Simenti staff and field assistants for their support in the field, in particular Djibril Coly, Chérif Younousse Kéba Camara, and Amadou Bamba Diedhiou. We owe special thanks to Irene Gutiérrez Díez for her help with the data collection in the field.
Funding
Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL. This research was supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, German Research Foundation), Grant/Award Number: 254142454 / GRK 2070.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
LO, JF and DZ designed the study. JB equipped Guinea baboon males with GPS collars and provided the description of the collaring procedure. LO collected the data in 2022. RM and LO analysed the data and prepared the figures. LO prepared the manuscript with contributions from all co-authors. All authors read, edited and approved the final manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Ohrndorf, L., Mundry, R., Beckmann, J. et al. Impact of food availability and predator presence on patterns of landscape partitioning among neighbouring Guinea baboon (Papio papio) parties. Mov Ecol 13, 9 (2025). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40462-025-00534-9
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40462-025-00534-9