Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

Predictors of alcohol use disorder risk in young adults: Direct and indirect psychological paths through binge drinking

  • Maxime Mauduy ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Resources, Software, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    maxime.mauduy@u-paris.fr

    Affiliation Laboratoire de Psychologie Sociale (LPS, UR 4471), University of Paris Cité, Boulogne-Billancourt, France

  • Pierre Maurage,

    Roles Methodology, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Louvain Experimental Psychopathology Research Group (LEP), Psychological Science Research Institute, UCLouvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium

  • Nicolas Mauny,

    Roles Data curation, Investigation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Laboratoire de Psychologie (UR 3188), University of Franche-Comté, Besançon, France

  • Anne-Lise Pitel,

    Roles Resources, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Physiopathology and Imaging of Neurological Disorders, University of Caen Normandy, INSERM, Caen, France

  • Hélène Beaunieux,

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Project administration, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Laboratoire de Psychologie Caen Normandie (LPCN, UR 7452), University of Caen Normandy, Caen, France

  • Jessica Mange

    Roles Conceptualization, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Laboratoire de Psychologie Caen Normandie (LPCN, UR 7452), University of Caen Normandy, Caen, France

Abstract

Alcohol-use disorders (AUD) risk is highly prevalent in university students, and is associated with both intraindividual (e.g., metacognitions, personality traits) and interindividual (e.g., social motives, drinking identity, drinking norms) psychological factors. Binge drinking (BD) also constitutes a widespread drinking pattern in youth, distinct from AUD risk and mainly predicted by interindividual factors. As BD is itself a risk factor for AUD, we tested a dual psychological path model to AUD risk, combining a direct path (including intra/interindividual factors independent from BD) with an indirect path (where interindividual factors increase AUD risk through BD). We assessed a large range of psychological factors predicting BD and AUD risk in an online survey among 2026 university students (Mage = 20.46; SD = 2.82; 67.42% of women). We tested the direct and indirect (via BD) effects of these psychological factors on three subdimensions of AUDIT (alcohol intake, dependence symptoms, and alcohol-related problems) through a multivariate mediation model using percentile bootstrapped estimates. Support for the dual-path model to risk of AUD emerged from the results, comprising a direct path mainly influenced by intra-individual factors unrelated to BD (e.g., coping motives, depression symptoms, and uncontrollability beliefs), and an indirect BD-mediated path mainly influenced by inter-individual factors (e.g., social motives, enhancement motives, drinking norms) through BD. This new dual-path conceptualization combining direct/intra-individual and indirect/inter-individual risk factors identifies key psychological determinants of AUD risk in youth and offers new prevention avenues for AUD risk.

Introduction

The prevalence of Alcohol Use Disorder (AUD) risk among university students is alarmingly high, with more than half of this population scoring above the threshold of seven/eight on the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT) [14]. Despite this, an integrated model that delineates the key intra-individual and inter-individual psychological variables contributing to AUD risk, particularly in the context of binge drinking (BD), remains absent. BD, defined as a drinking pattern characterized by alternating episodes of heavy alcohol consumption and periods of abstinence [5], is widespread among university students (e.g., 65.1% of students in a French sample presented occasional or frequent BD) and a known risk factor for the development of AUD [68]. Moreover, the psychological mechanisms underlying AUD risk in university students appear closely tied to those associated with BD, suggesting that the progression to AUD in this population often begins with BD. However, alcohol dependence symptoms and its associated problems, two major indictors of AUD, are distinct constructs from heavy consumption levels like BD [911]. This raises the possibility that other psychological mechanisms, beyond those associated with BD, may contribute to AUD risk in this population. Thus, this study seeks to refine our understanding of the pathways leading to AUD risk among university students by investigating the relationships between psychological factors, BD, and AUD risk. We hypothesize that certain psychological factors, depending on whether they are intra-individual or inter-individual, may exert direct effects on AUD risk, while others influence AUD risk indirectly via BD.

Inter- and intra-individual psychological factors associated with AUD risk

A substantial body of research has identified psychological factors associated with AUD risk in university students, which can be broadly categorized as either inter-individual or intra-individual [12,13].

Three major inter-individual factors have consistently been linked to AUD risk in young populations. First, drinking social identity - the extent to which individuals identify themselves as part of the social category of “drinkers” [14,15] - positively predicts alcohol use and AUD risk among youth [1518]. Second, based on Cooper’s motivational model [19], two external drinking motives, namely social (i.e., positive external motives such as drinking to enhance social interactions) and conformity (i.e., negative external motives such as drinking to avoid social censure or rejection) motives have emerged as strong predictors of alcohol use [16,20] and AUD risk [19,2123]. Notably, higher levels of social motives are positively associated with AUD risk, while conformity motives are negatively related to AUD risk. Third, drinking social norms - individuals’ beliefs about the acceptability and prevalence of alcohol use among peers [24], influence alcohol use [25] and AUD risk [26]. Specifically, more positive perceptions of peer drinking are associated with alcohol consumption and increased AUD risk.

Three key intra-individual factors also contribute to AUD risk in young people. First, according to Cooper’s motivational model, two internal drinking motives, namely enhancement (i.e., positive internal motive such as drinking to elevate positive mood) and coping (i.e., negative internal motives such as drinking to regulate negative affect) motives are associated with greater alcohol use and AUD risk in young people [12,16,20,27]. Second, alcohol-related metacognitions - schematic information that individuals hold about the significance of their cognitive experience and ways to control it - are significant predictors of AUD risk [28,29]. These metacognitions include positive beliefs about the impact of alcohol use on cognitive (helping to control thoughts) and emotional (helping to improve mood) self-regulation, and negative metacognitions related to the perceived uncontrollability and the negative cognitive functioning impact of alcohol use. Third, personality traits and psychopathological variables, such as loneliness (i.e., a distressing feeling of isolation perception or social rejection [30,31]), anxiety [32], depression symptoms [33], and impulsivity (i.e., the tendency to act prematurely without considering consequences) have been consistently associated with alcohol use [34,35] and AUD risk [31,36,37]. More precisely, impulsivity, as conceptualized in the UPPS model [38], encompasses dimensions such as negative and positive urgency (i.e., the tendency to act rashly in intense negative or positive emotional contexts), lack of perseverance (i.e., the difficulty to maintain attention on a task), lack of premeditation (i.e., the difficulty to plan behaviors and consider their consequences), and sensation seeking (i.e., the propensity to look for new and exciting experiences), all of which have been linked to alcohol-related outcomes [39].

However, it is worth noting that these psychological determinants of AUD risk, and particularly drinking social identity and enhancement motives, are also the strongest predictors of BD [12,26], suggesting that BD and AUD may share common psychological predictors, that an indirect psychological pathway might lead to AUD through BD, and that preventing BD could reduce AUD risk among students.

An indirect psychological pathway from BD to AUD

Social factors are widely recognized as primary initiators of alcohol use in young adults [4043], a highly social behavior [44], characterized by the BD practice, a socially motivated drinking pattern [45]. Indeed, theoretical models, such as Koob and Volkow’s three-stage conceptualization of addiction [46] and the socioecological framework of drinking contexts [47], emphasize that excessive alcohol consumption, and particularly BD, is a critical precursor to AUD development. Consistent with these models, research shows that BD is a significant risk factor for AUD [6,7,48].

BD, as a social drinking pattern, is primarily driven by psychological factors that can be classified into inter-individual (e.g., drinking social identity, drinking norms, social motives) and intra-individual (e.g., positive enhancement motives, sensation seeking) categories. These factors align with the positive affect regulation theory of AUD etiology, which conceptualizes AUD as a maladaptive strategy to enhance positive emotions and social experiences [40]. According to this theory, positive alcohol expectancies - beliefs about alcohol’s ability to improve emotional states and facilitate social bonding - encourage individuals to consume alcohol and increase the likelihood of developing AUD. Supporting this perspective, studies have demonstrated that these BD-related psychological factors, especially interpersonal ones, are indirectly linked to AUD symptoms through higher levels of alcohol consumption [21,49,50].

Consequently, rather than exerting a direct influence on AUD risk, we hypothesize that these psychological factors primarily promote BD, which in turn increases AUD risk in young adults. In other words, psychological factors associated with the positive affect regulation conceptualization might lead young adults to engage in BD, which subsequently increases their AUD risk. However, existing studies often fail to account for this indirect pathway - and therefore report direct associations - due to two key methodological limitations.

Methodological limitations affecting the understanding of AUD risk

First, research investigating psychological determinants of AUD typically examines these factors in isolation from BD, focusing on their direct effects on AUD risk without considering the mediating role of BD. This approach overlooks the possibility that BD may serve as a mediator between some psychological factors (e.g., drinking identity, social motives) and AUD risk [21,50], potentially explaining why many determinants appear common to both BD and AUD risk.

Second, most studies predicting AUD risk among university students use the overall AUDIT score as the outcome variable. According to the original authors, AUDIT measures three major dimensions associated with AUD risk: alcohol intake, alcohol dependence symptoms, and alcohol-related problems [51]. However, these three sub-dimensions (i) correspond to different aspects of alcohol consumption [9,10,51,52], (ii) have different predictors and causal factors [53], and (iii) are themselves different predictors of drinking behavior and its consequences [54]. Notably, the “alcohol intake” dimension is highly correlated with BD, as evidenced by the sensitivity and specificity of AUDIT-C in detecting BD [55,56]. In contrast, the other two dimensions - dependence symptoms and alcohol-related problems - capture distinct aspects of AUD risk from excessive consumption [9,10]. Consequently, studies relying on the overall AUDIT score may overestimate the role of BD-related psychological determinants in AUD risk and underestimate factors more closely tied to dependence symptoms and alcohol-related problems. Therefore, a growing body of evidence suggests that the AUDIT is not a unitary scale but should rather be distinguished according to its sub-dimensions to investigate the risk of AUD [911,57,58].

Thus, these two methodological issues may lead to (i) an overestimation of AUD predictors that are shared with BD but primarily act indirectly through BD, and (ii) an underestimation of predictors less correlated with BD but directly influencing AUD risk. Indeed, when focusing specifically on dependence symptoms and alcohol-related problems, some evidence suggests that specific psychological factors are associated with AUD risk among youth, highlighting a direct psychological pathway to AUD, independent from BD.

A direct psychological pathway to AUD risk

One of the most prominent theories of AUD etiology suggests that AUD develops as a way to alleviate negative affect, such as reducing anxiety, stress, and depression [27,5962]. Many young adults report drinking to cope with negative emotions [26,63,64]. Psychological factors associated with this negative affect regulation process - such as negative reinforcement motives (i.e., coping motives), negative alcohol-related metacognitions, and specific personality traits - may play a direct role in AUD risk, independent of BD, which would be less influenced by these factors. Empirical evidence supports this perspective. Studies focusing on the prediction of dependence symptoms and alcohol-related problems - rather than overall AUDIT scores - reveal notable differences between psychological predictors of BD and those of AUD risk.

Regarding drinking motives, while BD is primarily explained by social and enhancement motives, coping motives are more strongly associated with alcohol-related problems [27,65] and dependence symptoms [32,66]. Thus, coping motives appear to exert a direct influence on AUD risk, independent of BD practice.

Regarding alcohol-related metacognitions, empirical research highlights distinct roles for positive and negative metacognitions. Positive metacognitions, such as emotional self-regulation, are primarily associated with BD [28]. Conversely, negative metacognitions (e.g., beliefs about uncontrollability and negative consequences of alcohol use) are crucial for the persistence of alcohol consumption [67,68] and are significant predictors of alcohol dependence [69]. These findings suggest that negative metacognitions play a direct role in AUD risk among university students, independent of BD practice.

Regarding personality traits, the predictive role of impulsivity dimensions differs between alcohol use and alcohol-related problems or dependence symptoms. While the lack of perseverance is more strongly associated with alcohol use, negative urgency and the lack of premeditation are more predictive of AUD risk [39]. Additionally, loneliness is a characteristic specific to higher AUD risk compared to lower dependence risk [31], and depression symptoms have been more strongly associated with alcohol dependence symptoms than with general alcohol use [33]. These findings indicate that some impulsivity dimensions (e.g., negative urgency and lack of premeditation), as well as loneliness and depression, may directly contribute to AUD risk among university students, beyond their association with BD.

The present research

The direct psychological determinants of AUD risk in university students, particularly concerning its dimensions of dependence symptoms and alcohol-related problems, may differ in nature from those of BD. We propose that the psychological factors commonly associated with both AUD and BD risk in prior research may primarily reflect the mediating role of BD itself as a risk factor for AUD - a relationship often unaccounted for in existing statistical models. To address this gap, we hypothesize a dual-pathway model of AUD risk (see Fig 1), wherein:

thumbnail
Fig 1. The hypothesis of a psychological dual-path model of Risk of Alcohol Use Disorders in University students.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0321974.g001

  1. (1). psychological factors less correlated with BD (e.g., coping motives, negative metacognitions, impulsivity dimensions, loneliness, and depression), mainly intra-individual in nature, exert a direct influence on AUD risk, independent of BD;
  2. (2). psychological determinants of BD (e.g., drinking identity, social norms, social motives, enhancement motives, and sensation seeking), mainly inter-individual in nature, contribute indirectly to AUD risk, through the mediation of BD.

The first direct psychological pathway thus reflects the negative affect regulation conceptualization of AUD, while the second indirect psychological pathway primarily reflects the positive affect regulation conceptualization of AUD. Establishing this dual-pathway model could be critical for developing effective reduction strategies for AUD risk among university students by providing a nuanced understanding of the psychological mechanisms underlying AUD risk among youth. Accordingly, we tested this dual-pathway hypothesis in a large sample of university students.

Method

For more details on the method, see the supporting information.

Procedure and participants

We contacted 27,668 students from the university of Caen Normandy (France) through their institutional e-mail address to take part in an online survey (via the Limesurvey® application, from the beginning to the end of November 2019). The only inclusion criterion for taking part in the study was to be a Caen Normandie University student of legal age (18). We obtained 3,939 responses, this response rate (14.2%) being in line with previous studies in this population [7073]. We then selected 2,026 eligible answers (i.e., students drinking alcohol regularly and having answered to the items related to alcohol consumption and psychological variables) which constituted the sample of the present study (see Fig 2 for the flow diagram and Table 1 for sample characteristics). Our sample is comparable in terms of socio-demographic data, alcohol consumption and psychopathological variables to those of previous studies exploring alcohol consumption among European university students [1,26,28,7378]. This study was included in a larger research project exploring substance consumption among young adults (ADUC project: “Alcool et Drogues à l’Université de Caen”).

Ethics

All participants gave their written consent before starting the survey. The study was notified to and authorized by the “Commission Nationale de l’Informatique et des Libertés” with the registration number u24- 20171109-01R1. This survey was conducted in full agreement with the Declaration of Helsinki (2008) and the ethical standards set by the Psychology Department, that follows the American Psychological Association Ethical Principles of Psychologists and the Code of Conduct (APA, 2019) for the ethical treatment of human participants.

Measures

As our measures use four, five or seven points Likert-type scales, we have better assessed the reliability of the measures by using an ordinal coefficient alpha, rather than the classic Cronbach alpha [79].

Sociodemographic variables.

We measured participants’ gender and age.

Alcohol consumption variables.

We computed a BD score using three questions (i.e., Q1: “number of average standard drinks (corresponding to 10 gr of ethanol in France) per hour”, Q2: “number of times being drunk in the previous 12 months” and Q3: “percentage of times getting drunk when drinking” [12,80]. The computed BD score (i.e., [4 × Q1] + Q2 + [0.2 × Q3]) [81] allows us to consider both quantity and frequency of BD [45,81].

We measured risk of AUD from the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT52). The AUDIT is a 10-item measure designed to identify individuals at risk for AUD by assessing three subdimensions [52,66]: alcohol intake (items 1–3, frequency of drinking, number of drinks consumed on a typical day, frequency of heavy drinking), alcohol dependence symptoms (items 4–6, impaired control over drinking, increased salience of drinking, morning drinking), and alcohol related-problems (items 7–10, guilt after drinking, blackouts, alcohol-related injuries, others concerned about drinking). We tested the dimensionality of the AUDIT using confirmatory factor analyses. While the model comprising a single dimension (overall AUDIT score) showed a poor fit to the data (χ²(35) = 1128, p < .001, CFI = .852, TLI = .810, RMSEA = .105, 90% CI [.10,.11], SRMR = .049), the model distinguishing the three AUDIT sub-dimensions showed an excellent fit to the data (χ²(32) = 383, p < .001, CFI = .953, TLI = .933, RMSEA = .062, 90% CI [.057,.068], SRMR = .035). This reinforces the idea of avoiding the use of an overall AUDIT score to rather measure each of the three AUDIT subdimensions separately to investigate AUD risk. Thus, we computed a score for alcohol intake (α = 0.73), a score for alcohol dependence symptoms (α = 0.81), and a score for alcohol-related problems (α = 0.76).

Inter-individual psychological predictors.

Drinking identity, that is the extent to which alcohol use is important to define the participant’s identity, was assessed with a 2-item Likert-type scale from 1 = do not agree to 4 = agree very much; α = 0.83 [12,82].

Drinking social norms, namely how much most of the participants’ significant relatives approve and/or adopt alcohol consumption to “get drunk”, were assessed with a 2-item Likert-type scale from 1 = do not agree to 7 = agree very much and one item from 1 = no person to 6 = 5 persons (α = 0.89) [12].

Drinking motives are the individuals’ reasons for engaging in alcohol use and were assessed with the Drinking Motives Questionnaire (DMQ-R) [83]. It is a 12-item scale rated on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 = never to 5 = always, including, as interindividual motives, social (α = 0.90) and conformity (α = 0.90) subscales, and as intraindividual motives, coping (α = 0.92), enhancement (α = 0.85) subscales.

Intra-individual psychological predictors.

Alcohol-related metacognitions were assessed using the French version of the Positive Alcohol Metacognitions Scale (PAMS) and the Negative Alcohol Metacognitions Scale (NAMS; Likert-type scales from 1 = do not agree to 4 = agree very much) [28]. We measured metacognitions about emotional (i.e., drinking helps to improve mood; 8-item, α = 0.92) and cognitive (i.e., drinking helps to control thoughts; 4-item, α = 0.80) self-regulation and uncontrollability (i.e., perceived loss of control due to alcohol use; 3-item, α = 0.76) and cognitive harm (i.e., perceived negative cognitive functioning impact of alcohol use; 3-item, Cronbach α = 0.81).

Impulsivity was measured using the French short version of the UPPS-P Impulsive Behavior Scale [84]. It is a 20-item Likert-type scale ranging from 1 = do not agree to 4 = agree very much. We measured five facets of impulsivity, namely positive (α = 0.81) and negative (α = 0.85) urgency (i.e., the tendency to act rashly to regulate negative and positive emotions), lack of premeditation (i.e., the tendency to act without thinking; α = 0.86), lack of perseverance (i.e., the tendency to not finish tasks; α = 0.92), and sensation seeking (i.e., the tendency to seek out new or thrilling experience; α = 0.86).

Anxiety was measured with the French version of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) [85]. It is a 20-item scale ranging from (1) no to (4) yes (α = .94).

Depression symptoms were assessed with the short French version of the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) [86]. It is a 13-item scale where participants answer (1) yes or (0) no.

Loneliness was measured with the ESUL (i.e., “Echelle de Solitude de l’Université de Laval”), a Canadian-French speaking adaptation of the UCLA Loneliness Scale [87]. It is a 20-item scale ranging ranging from 1 = never to 4 = often (α = 0.95).

Statistical analysis

We conducted a multivariate mediation model with psychological factors as predictors, BD behavior as the mediator, and the three subdimensions of AUDIT as outcomes.

To account for gender effects, we included gender as a covariate in the model tested (coded as Female: -0.5; Male: 0.5). Gender was significantly associated with BD behavior (β = .10, 95% CI [.07,.14]), drinking identity (β = .18, 95% CI [.14,.23]), social motives (β = .12, 95% CI [.08,.17]), enhancement motives (β = .07, 95% CI [.03,.11]), conformity motives (β = -.05, 95% CI [-.09, -.007]), drinking norms (β = .11, 95% CI [.06,.15]), negative urgency (β = -.15, 95% CI [-.19, -.11]), positive urgency (β = -.09, 95% CI [-.13, -.04]), lack of perseverance (β = .07, 95% CI [.03,.11]), sensation seeking (β = .15, 95% CI [.11,.19]), anxiety (β = -.26, 95% CI [-.30, -.22]), depression symptoms (β = -.15, 95% CI [-.19, -.11]), emotional self-regulation (β = -.06, 95% CI [.01,.10]), cognitive self-regulation (β = .06, 95% CI [.01,.10]), and cognitive harm (β = -.04, 95% CI [-.09, -.001]).

Given the non-normality observed in several variables - common in drinking behavior data due to positive skewness and/or kurtosis [88] - we applied a log10 transformation to the following variables: cognitive self-regulation, uncontrollability beliefs, drinking identity, conformity motives, binge drinking, and alcohol dependence symptoms. To further address potential violations of multivariate normality, we utilized maximum likelihood estimation combined with non-parametric percentile bootstrapping (N = 10,000 resamples) to estimate path coefficients and 95% Confidence Intervals (CIs). This bootstrapping approach effectively mitigates issues related to non-normal data distributions [89,90]. As the bootstrapping approach does not allow us to deal with missing data, our statistical model carried out on our sample of 2,026 students did not include any missing data.

The model tested both direct effects of the psychological predictors on the AUDIT subdimensions (alcohol intake, dependence symptoms, and alcohol-related problems) and their indirect effects mediated by BD. To determine the presence of mediation effects, we adhered to the criteria outlined by Yzerbyt et al. [91], which require that: the effect of the predictor on the mediator (a), the effect of the mediator on the outcome (b), and the indirect effect (a*b) must all be significant. All statistical analyses were performed using R software (version 4.05), specifically employing the mvn [92] and lavaan [93] packages.

Results

Fig 3 illustrates the main results of the analyses.

thumbnail
Fig 3. Results of the psychological dual-path model of Risk of Alcohol Use Disorders in University students according to the three AUDIT dimensions.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0321974.g003

Direct predictors of AUDIT subdimensions

We identified a direct pathway to AUD risk, predominantly influenced by intraindividual predictors (see Table 2). As expected, beyond the direct effect of BD, alcohol dependence symptoms and alcohol-related problems are more predicted by intraindividual factors - such as coping motives, metacognitions related to cognitive self-regulation, uncontrollability beliefs and cognitive harm, lack of perseverance, loneliness, and depression symptoms - than by interindividual factors, which include drinking identity and conformity motives. In contrast, alcohol intake was associated with both interindividual factors (i.e., drinking identity, conformity motives, and social motives) and intraindividual factors (i.e., coping motives, cognitive harm, and negative urgency). Additionally, other factors, such as enhancement motives and drinking norms, did not show a direct association with any of the AUDIT subdimensions.

thumbnail
Table 2. Bootstrap analyses of direct effects for factors predicting the three subdimensions of AUDIT and binge drinking.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0321974.t002

Indirect predictors of AUDIT subdimensions through BD

Results indicated that BD is mostly predicted by inter-individual factors including drinking identity, social motives, enhancement motives, conformity motives, and drinking social norms, but also by some intraindividual factors, such as emotional self-regulation, lack of premeditation, sensation seeking, and loneliness (see Table 2). Furthermore, nearly all of these direct predictors of BD demonstrated significant indirect effects on the three AUDIT subdimensions through BD behavior, with the exception of loneliness and lack of premeditation, which did not significantly influence alcohol dependence symptoms (see Table 3).

thumbnail
Table 3. Bootstrap analyses of indirect effects for psychological variables predicting the three subdimensions of AUDIT via binge drinking behavior.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0321974.t003

Discussion

We aimed to deepen the understanding of the psychological determinants of AUD risk in university students by addressing two key gaps in previous research. First, we explored the possibility of a dual psychological pathway to AUD risk: a direct path, primarily driven by intraindividual factors, and an indirect path, mediated by BD - a known risk factor for AUD - predominantly involving interindividual factors. Second, we distinguished between the three subdimensions of the AUDIT - alcohol intake, alcohol dependence symptoms, and alcohol-related problems - since their direct psychological determinants may differ. Our study, therefore, investigated this dual psychological pathway to AUD risk in a sample of university students.

Evidence of a dual pathway to AUD risk in university students

Our results first identified a direct pathway to AUD risk, independent of BD. Overall, AUD risk is predominantly predicted by intraindividual factors such as alcohol-related metacognitions and impulsivity, rather than by interindividual factors like drinking social norms. This direct intraindividual pathway aligns with existing theoretical frameworks and empirical findings, which emphasize the significant role of intraindividual and personality factors in AUD, particularly in relation to dependence symptoms and alcohol-related problems [32,39,66,68,94]. Moreover, we demonstrated that this direct pathway to AUD risk is distinct from the one leading to BD, which is primarily influenced by interindividual factors (e.g., drinking identity, social motives, drinking norms). This distinction supports previous research on BD determinants [12], reinforcing the idea that BD is fundamentally a positive reinforcement social drinking pattern [12,80], while AUD risk is more closely related to negative regulatory strategies [19,63,9597], often involving negative reinforcement motives (e.g., coping motives, cognitive self-regulation) associated with negative thoughts (e.g., uncontrollability and cognitive harm beliefs) used to manage psychological distress (e.g., depression, loneliness).

Second, our findings reveal that traditional interindividual factors [12,66,98], such as social motives, conformity motives, and drinking social norms, are largely indirect determinants of AUD risk in university students. Their influence on AUD risk, particularly regarding alcohol dependence symptoms and alcohol-related problems, is mediated by BD. While these findings are consistent with previous research identifying BD as a risk factor for AUD [6,7], they underscore the existence of an indirect psychological pathway to AUD risk, mediated by BD. This research results do not challenge the notion that social factors, particularly social norms, play a significant role in AUD and alcohol-related problems. Extensive evidence demonstrates that social norms influence the negative consequences of alcohol consumption [99,100] and that prevention strategies targeting these norms can effectively reduce such consequences [101]. However, our results rather suggest that the influence of social norms may be more directly related to excessive alcohol consumption behaviors (i.e., BD) than to dependence symptoms or the associated negative outcomes, consistently with some previous studies [47,102]. Social factors may serve as a gateway to alcohol consumption among young people, encouraging heavy social drinking behaviors such as BD, which in turn lead to negative consequences. The risk of AUD among young people appears to arise indirectly, as their normative social environment promotes BD, thereby contributing to the emergence of dependence symptoms and alcohol-related problems along this indirect psychological pathway. Thus, this indirect pathway clarifies the role of interindividual factors and BD as predictors of AUD in university students.

Our results thus support the dual-pathway model to AUD risk in university students, with an indirect pathway via BD, primarily consisting of interindividual factors and accounting for 37.12% of alcohol intake, 20% of dependence symptoms, and 22.61% of alcohol-related problems, and a direct pathway, primarily consisting of intraindividual factors. This is in line with a recent conceptualization of AUD risk in young people that also considers two pathways to AUD [103]. The first pathway to AUD in young people would involve the social practice of alcohol consumption, predicted by psychosocial factors linked to the idea of alcohol consumption for positive reinforcement purposes. A second pathway to AUD in young people would involve solitary drinking, predicted by intra-individual psychological factors associated with the idea of drinking for negative reinforcement.

Therefore, future studies should further explore our dual pathway, particularly to identity the drinking patterns of youth who follow the direct pathway to AUD risk, and particularly the influence of solitary drinking. Additionally, it is important to determine whether these two pathways are mutually exclusive or whether they can coexist within the same individual. While our variable-centered statistical approach demonstrates the presence of both pathways as distinct and independent - each operating beyond the influence of the other - it also suggests that they may coexist. Supporting this notion, recent research suggests that these two pathways might operate simultaneously within individual. Youth solitary drinkers, who could instead take the direct path to AUD, still spend the majority of their time drinking in social settings [104,105]. Moreover, Lannoy et al. [20] identified a psychological profile of binge drinkers characterized by both positive reinforcement motives (e.g., social and enhancement) and negative ones (e.g., coping motives), while Lannoy et al. [71] identified a profile of alcohol users characterized by both positive and negative reinforcement motives (e.g., social and coping motives) and impulsivity traits (e.g., lack of premeditation and perseverance). These profiles were found to be at the highest risk of AUD compared to profiles characterized only by positive reinforcement motives (e.g., enhancement and social). Thus, both pathways might coexist in some students, amplifying their AUD risk, while others may only present the indirect pathway via BD. The existing literature has yet to identify a group of non-binge-drinking students characterized predominantly by intraindividual factors such as coping motives or personality traits. Thus, future research should better delineate this direct pathway to AUD risk in university students, including its relationship to consumption patterns that do not involve BD. Additionally, the hypothesis that the direct pathway may develop after the indirect one in some students warrants exploration through longitudinal studies, as it aligns with recent findings showing that drinking motives change over time and interact: social motives may increase coping motives, while coping motives may reduce enhancement motives [106]. Finally, although our research aimed to explore the major pathways of psychological factors that predict AUD risk, intra-individual and inter-individual factors may interact together to predict BD or AUD risk notably because each of these two alcohol outcomes is always influenced by a specific combination of inter-individual and intra-individual factors. Therefore, clarifying the interactions between these two types of factors within each pathway would be an interesting research perspective to pursue by using cluster or latent profile analyses.

Drinker identity as a key psychological factor in both pathways to AUD risk

Moreover, our results also underscore the significance of one key psychological factor: drinking identity. This factor uniquely predicts AUD both directly and indirectly through BD, consistent with previous studies on BD [12] and AUD [26]. This highlight the importance of drinking identity in preventive measures and encourage further investigation into its role in AUD. Drinking identity comprises two interconnected yet distinct aspects: social identity (i.e., viewing oneself as a member of a social category) [12] and personal identity (i.e., considering a behavior as a core part of the self) [97]. It is plausible that its dual role in predicting AUD risk directly and influencing it indirectly through BD reflects these two aspects: personal drinking identity for the direct pathway and social drinking identity for the indirect pathway to AUD risk.

Practical implications of the dual pathway model of AUD risk

Since BD is a highly prevalent drinking pattern among students and a risk factor for developing AUD, practitioners might focus on preventing this drinking behavior to mitigate the risk of AUD. To achieve this, prevention interventions should target the key psychological determinants of BD, and centrally social factors. Interventions addressing social norms through techniques like normative feedback [107] or induced hypocrisy [108], as well as interventions targeting drinker social identity using the multi-categorization technique [109], could prove effective in reducing this excessive alcohol consumption pattern and reduce the transition towards AUD.

However, the findings of this research, particularly the identification of the dual pathway, suggest that BD is not the sole route to AUD risk among young people. Other psychological factors, primarily intra-individual in nature and only weakly linked (or unrelated) to BD, also contribute to AUD risk, and more particularly the symptoms of dependence and the negative consequences associated with alcohol consumption. Therefore, it is crucial not to overlook, in the context of alcohol consumption among students, prevention interventions targeting coping drinking motivations, alcohol-related negative metacognitive beliefs, impulsivity and depressive symptoms. For instance, interventions focusing on coping motives with a framing technique associated with short-term perspective [110,111], metacognitive beliefs via persuasive communication [112], impulsivity through cognitive remediation techniques [113], and depressive symptoms via mindfulness-based interventions [114] could be effective in mitigating this direct psychological pathway toward AUD in students.

Thus, the findings of this research suggest that, rather than prioritizing interventions targeting either the indirect pathway to AUD or its direct pathway, designing a comprehensive intervention program that combines both sets of interventions would likely be the most effective approach to counter AUD risk among students.

Limitations and future directions

First, although previous longitudinal studies [66,115] support the direction of relationships posited in the dual-pathway model to AUD risk tested here, our study utilized a cross-sectional design. Second, the self-reported nature of the survey may have introduced recall and social desirability biases, though the anonymous nature of online surveys likely mitigated this bias to some extent. Third, we tested our dual-pathway model by considering all psychological factors at the same level of analysis. Future research should aim to refine this model by considering the interplay and mutual influences among psychological factors [27,98,115]. This single-level analysis may explain why some psychological factors (e.g., anxiety) known to influence alcohol use and AUD were not found to be significant in our study. Finally, while our results are consistent with previous studies [12,66] and while the model tested appears generalizable to other populations and contexts, beyond university students (since the variables tested are general determinants of alcohol consumption and AUD risk beyond specific contexts), their generalizability should be confirmed in future studies on more various samples.

Conclusion

We developed and tested a dual psychological pathway to AUD risk. On the one hand, we identified a direct pathway, primarily involving intraindividual factors (e.g., coping motives and personality traits), which directly influence alcohol dependence symptoms and alcohol-related problems, distinct from those associated with BD. On the other hand, we demonstrated an indirect pathway, predominantly based on interindividual factors (e.g., drinking norms, social motives), which predicts AUD risk indirectly through BD. This study enhances the understanding of AUD risk predictors in university students by clarifying the respective roles of intraindividual and interindividual factors by taking into account the role of BD. Highlighting this dual psychological pathway holds significant implications for prevention efforts. Indeed, it emphasizes that attention should not be solely directed towards addressing the key psychological factors of BD alone, as AUD risk among students does not solely stem from BD practices and may involve psychological factors distinct from those associated with BD.

Supporting information

References

  1. 1. Bollen Z, Pabst A, Creupelandt C, Fontesse S, Lannoy S, Pinon N, et al. Prior drinking motives predict alcohol consumption during the COVID-19 lockdown: a cross-sectional online survey among Belgian college students. Addict Behav. 2021;115:106772. pmid:33418433
  2. 2. Partington S, Partington E, Heather N, Longstaff F, Allsop S, Jankowski M, et al. The relationship between membership of a university sports group and drinking behaviour among students at English Universities. Addict Res Theory. 2012;21(4):339–47.
  3. 3. Rickwood D, George A, Parker R, Mikhailovich K. Harmful alcohol use on campus: impact on young people at university. Youth Stud Aust. 2011;30(1):34–40.
  4. 4. Tavolacci M-P, Boerg E, Richard L, Meyrignac G, Dechelotte P, Ladner J. Prevalence of binge drinking and associated behaviours among 3286 college students in France. BMC Public Health. 2016;16:178. pmid:26905284
  5. 5. National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism. NIAAA council approves definition of binge drinking. NIAAA Newsl. 2004;Winter(3):3.
  6. 6. Nieto SJ, Baskerville W, Donato S, Bujarski S, Ray L. Lifetime heavy drinking years predict alcohol use disorder severity over and above current alcohol use. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse. 2021;47(5):630–7. pmid:34134588
  7. 7. Tavolacci M-P, Berthon Q, Cerasuolo D, Dechelotte P, Ladner J, Baguet A. Does binge drinking between the age of 18 and 25 years predict alcohol dependence in adulthood? A retrospective case-control study in France. BMJ Open. 2019;9(5):e026375. pmid:31061035
  8. 8. Bonomo YA, Bowes G, Coffey C, Carlin JB, Patton GC. Teenage drinking and the onset of alcohol dependence: a cohort study over seven years. Addiction. 2004;99(12):1520–8. pmid:15585043
  9. 9. Horváth Z, Nagy L, Koós M, Kraus SW, Demetrovics Z, Potenza MN, et al. Psychometric properties of the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT) across cross-cultural subgroups, genders, and sexual orientations: Findings from the International Sex Survey (ISS). Compr Psychiatry. 2023;127:152427. pmid:37782987
  10. 10. Babor TF, Robaina K. The Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT): a review of graded severity algorithms and national adaptations. IJADR. 2016;5(2):17–24.
  11. 11. Gecaite-Stonciene J, Steibliene V, Fineberg NA, Podlipskyte A, Bunevicius A, Liaugaudaite V, et al. Multidimensional structure of the alcohol use disorders identification test: factorial validity and reliability in patients with anxiety and mood disorders in Lithuania. Alcohol Alcohol. 2021;56(1):109–15. pmid:33191431
  12. 12. Mange J, Mauduy M, Sénémeaud C, Bagneux V, Cabé N, Jacquet D, et al. What really matters in binge drinking: a dominance analysis of binge drinking psychological determinants among University students. Addict Behav Rep. 2021;13:100346. pmid:33997251
  13. 13. Sher KJ, Grekin ER, Williams NA. The development of alcohol use disorders. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2005;1:493–523. pmid:17716097
  14. 14. Hagger MS, Anderson M, Kyriakaki M, Darkings S. Aspects of identity and their influence on intentional behavior: comparing effects for three health behaviors. Pers Individ Differ. 2007;42(2):355–67.
  15. 15. Hertel AW, Peterson KP, Lindgren KP. Investment in drinking identity is associated with alcohol consumption and risk of alcohol use disorder. Addict Behav. 2019;89:256–62. pmid:30368029
  16. 16. Foster DW, Young CM, Bryan J, Steers M-LN, Yeung NCY, Prokhorov AV. Interactions among drinking identity, gender and decisional balance in predicting alcohol use and problems among college students. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2014;143:198–205. pmid:25127705
  17. 17. Meca A, Zamboanga BL, Kubilus R. Operationalizing drinking identity from a developmental perspective: the Drinking Identity Management Scale (DIMS). Identity. 2020;20(2):119–31.
  18. 18. Ramirez JJ, Fairlie AM, Olin CC, Lindgren KP. Implicit and explicit drinking identity predict latent classes that differ on the basis of college students’ drinking behaviors. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2017;178:579–85. pmid:28755560
  19. 19. Cooper ML. Motivations for alcohol use among adolescents: development and validation of a four-factor model. Psychol Assess. 1994;6(2):117–28.
  20. 20. Lannoy S, Billieux J, Poncin M, Maurage P. Binging at the campus: motivations and impulsivity influence binge drinking profiles in university students. Psychiatry Res. 2017;250:146–54. pmid:28161610
  21. 21. Magid V, Maclean MG, Colder CR. Differentiating between sensation seeking and impulsivity through their mediated relations with alcohol use and problems. Addict Behav. 2007;32(10):2046–61. pmid:17331658
  22. 22. Adams ZW, Kaiser AJ, Lynam DR, Charnigo RJ, Milich R. Drinking motives as mediators of the impulsivity-substance use relation: pathways for negative urgency, lack of premeditation, and sensation seeking. Addict Behav. 2012;37(7):848–55. pmid:22472524
  23. 23. Ham LS, Hope DA. College students and problematic drinking: a review of the literature. Clin Psychol Rev. 2003;23(5):719–59. pmid:12971907
  24. 24. Borsari B, Carey KB. Descriptive and injunctive norms in college drinking: a meta-analytic integration. J Stud Alcohol. 2003;64(3):331–41. pmid:12817821
  25. 25. Atwell K, Abraham C, Duka T. A parsimonious, integrative model of key psychological correlates of UK university students’ alcohol consumption. Alcohol Alcohol. 2011;46(3):253–60. pmid:21393342
  26. 26. Bakkali N, Mauduy M, Mange J, Maurage P, Grynberg D. Age-related variations of the psychosocial determinants of problematic alcohol consumption. Addict Res Theory. 2023;33(1):13–8.
  27. 27. Kuntsche E, Knibbe R, Gmel G, Engels R. Who drinks and why? A review of socio-demographic, personality, and contextual issues behind the drinking motives in young people. Addict Behav. 2006;31(10):1844–57. pmid:16460883
  28. 28. Gierski F, Spada MM, Fois E, Picard A, Naassila M, Van der Linden M. Positive and negative metacognitions about alcohol use among university students: psychometric properties of the PAMS and NAMS French versions. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2015;153:78–85. pmid:26094183
  29. 29. Moneta GB. Metacognition, emotion, and alcohol dependence in college students: a moderated mediation model. Addict Behav. 2011;36(7):781–4. pmid:21420790
  30. 30. Varga S, Piko BF. Being lonely or using substances with friends? A cross-sectional study of Hungarian adolescents’ health risk behaviours. BMC Public Health. 2015;15:1107. pmid:26547688
  31. 31. Wakabayashi M, Sugiyama Y, Takada M, Kinjo A, Iso H, Tabuchi T. Loneliness and increased hazardous alcohol use: data from a nationwide internet survey with 1-year follow-up. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2022;19(19):12086. pmid:36231395
  32. 32. Tragesser SL, Trull TJ, Sher KJ, Park A. Drinking motives as mediators in the relation between personality disorder symptoms and alcohol use disorder. J Pers Disord. 2008;22(5):525–37. pmid:18834299
  33. 33. Adewuya AO. Prevalence of major depressive disorder in Nigerian college students with alcohol-related problems. Gen Hosp Psychiatry. 2006;28(2):169–73. pmid:16516068
  34. 34. Krank M, Stewart SH, O’Connor R, Woicik PB, Wall A-M, Conrod PJ. Structural, concurrent, and predictive validity of the Substance Use Risk Profile Scale in early adolescence. Addict Behav. 2011;36(1–2):37–46. pmid:20826056
  35. 35. Quinn PD, Fromme K. Alcohol use and related problems among college students and their noncollege peers: the competing roles of personality and peer influence. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2011;72(4):622–32. pmid:21683044
  36. 36. Brar A, Moneta GB. Negative emotions and alcohol dependence symptoms in British Indian and White college students. Addict Behav. 2009;34(3):292–6. pmid:19070970
  37. 37. Malmberg M, Kleinjan M, Overbeek G, Vermulst AA, Lammers J, Engels RCME. Are there reciprocal relationships between substance use risk personality profiles and alcohol or tobacco use in early adolescence? Addict Behav. 2013;38(12):2851–9. pmid:24018230
  38. 38. Whiteside SP, Lynam DR. The Five Factor Model and impulsivity: using a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity. Pers Individ Differ. 2001;30(4):669–89.
  39. 39. Coskunpinar A, Dir AL, Cyders MA. Multidimensionality in impulsivity and alcohol use: a meta-analysis using the UPPS model of impulsivity. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2013;37(9):1441–50. pmid:23578176
  40. 40. Cooper ML, Kuntsche E, Levitt A, Barber LL, Wolf S. Motivational models of substance use. Vol 1. Sher KJ, editor. Oxford University Press; 2015. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199381678.013.017
  41. 41. Trucco EM. A review of psychosocial factors linked to adolescent substance use. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2020;196:172969. pmid:32565241
  42. 42. Maggs JL, Schulenberg JE. Initiation and course of alcohol consumption among adolescents and young adults. Recent Dev Alcohol. 2005;17:29–47. pmid:15789858
  43. 43. Mundt MP. The impact of peer social networks on adolescent alcohol use initiation. Acad Pediatr. 2011;11(5):414–21. pmid:21795133
  44. 44. Schulenberg JE, Maggs JL. A developmental perspective on alcohol use and heavy drinking during adolescence and the transition to young adulthood. J Stud Alcohol Suppl. 2002;5(s14):54–70. pmid:12022730
  45. 45. Maurage P, Lannoy S, Mange J, Grynberg D, Beaunieux H, Banovic I, et al. What we talk about when we talk about binge drinking: towards an integrated conceptualization and evaluation. Alcohol Alcohol. 2020;55(5):468–79. pmid:32556202
  46. 46. Koob GF, Volkow ND. Neurocircuitry of addiction. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35(1):217–38. pmid:19710631
  47. 47. Freisthler B, Lipperman-Kreda S, Bersamin M, Gruenewald PJ. Tracking the when, where, and with whom of alcohol use: integrating ecological momentary assessment and geospatial data to examine risk for alcohol-related problems. Alcohol Res. 2014;36(1):29–38. pmid:26258998
  48. 48. Patrick ME, Lee CM, Larimer ME. Drinking motives, protective behavioral strategies, and experienced consequences: identifying students at risk. Addict Behav. 2011;36(3):270–3. pmid:21159445
  49. 49. Reed MB, Lange JE, Ketchie JM, Clapp JD. The relationship between social identity, normative information, and college student drinking. Soc Influ. 2007;2(4):269–94.
  50. 50. Simons JS, Carey KB. An affective and cognitive model of marijuana and alcohol problems. Addict Behav. 2006;31(9):1578–92. pmid:16426771
  51. 51. Saunders JB, Aasland OG, Babor TF, de la Fuente JR, Grant M. Development of the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT): WHO collaborative project on early detection of persons with harmful alcohol consumption--II. Addiction. 1993;88(6):791–804. pmid:8329970
  52. 52. Gache P, Michaud P, Landry U, Accietto C, Arfaoui S, Wenger O, et al. The Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT) as a screening tool for excessive drinking in primary care: reliability and validity of a French version. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2005;29(11):2001–7. pmid:16340457
  53. 53. Simons JS, Carey KB, Wills TA. Alcohol abuse and dependence symptoms: a multidimensional model of common and specific etiology. Psychol Addict Behav. 2009;23(3):415–27. pmid:19769426
  54. 54. Conigrave KM, Saunders JB, Reznik RB. Predictive capacity of the AUDIT questionnaire for alcohol-related harm. Addiction. 1995;90(11):1479–85. pmid:8528033
  55. 55. García Carretero MÁ, Novalbos Ruiz JP, Martínez Delgado JM, O’Ferrall González C. Validation of the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test in university students: AUDIT and AUDIT-C. Adicciones. 2016;28(4):194–204. pmid:26990260
  56. 56. Cortés Tomás MT, Giménez Costa JA, Motos-Sellés P, Sancerni Beitia MD, Cadaveira Mahía F. The utility of the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT)for the analysis of binge drinking in university students. Psicothema. 2017;29(2):229–35. pmid:28438247
  57. 57. Moehring A, Krause K, Guertler D, Bischof G, Hapke U, Freyer-Adam J, et al. Measurement invariance of the alcohol use disorders identification test: Establishing its factor structure in different settings and across gender. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2018;189:55–61. pmid:29879682
  58. 58. Peng C-Z, Wilsnack RW, Kristjanson AF, Benson P, Wilsnack SC. Gender differences in the factor structure of the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test in multinational general population surveys. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2012;124(1–2):50–6. pmid:22236536
  59. 59. Baker TB, Piper ME, McCarthy DE, Majeskie MR, Fiore MC. Addiction motivation reformulated: an affective processing model of negative reinforcement. Psychol Rev. 2004;111(1):33–51. pmid:14756584
  60. 60. Khantzian EJ. Self-medication hypothesis. In: The SAGE encyclopedia of abnormal and clinical psychology. SAGE Publications, Inc.; 2017. p. 3045–6. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781483365817
  61. 61. Greeley J, Oei T. Alcohol and tension reduction. In: Psychological theories of drinking and alcoholism, 2nd ed. The Guilford substance abuse series. The Guilford Press; 1999. p. 14–53.
  62. 62. Skrzynski CJ, Creswell KG. Associations between solitary drinking and increased alcohol consumption, alcohol problems, and drinking to cope motives in adolescents and young adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Addiction. 2020;115(11):1989–2007. pmid:32196794
  63. 63. Cooper ML, Frone MR, Russell M, Mudar P. Drinking to regulate positive and negative emotions: a motivational model of alcohol use. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1995;69(5):990–1005. pmid:7473043
  64. 64. Sher KJ, Talley AE, Littlefield AK, Martinez JA. Alcohol use and alcohol use disorders. In: The Oxford handbook of health psychology. Oxford library of psychology. Oxford University Press; 2011. p. 686–737.
  65. 65. Kuntsche E, Knibbe R, Gmel G, Engels R. Why do young people drink? A review of drinking motives. Clin Psychol Rev. 2005;25(7):841–61. pmid:16095785
  66. 66. Fisher S, Hsu W-W, Adams Z, Arsenault C, Milich R. The effect of impulsivity and drinking motives on alcohol outcomes in college students: a 3-year longitudinal analysis. J Am Coll Health. 2022;70(6):1624–33. pmid:33048641
  67. 67. Spada MM, Wells A. Metacognitions about alcohol use in problem drinkers. Clin Psychology and Psychoth. 2006;13(2):138–43.
  68. 68. Spada MM, Caselli G, Nikčević AV, Wells A. Metacognition in addictive behaviors. Addict Behav. 2015;44:9–15. pmid:25182375
  69. 69. Spada MM, Wells A. Metacognitions across the continuum of drinking behaviour. Pers Individ Differ. 2010;49(5):425–9.
  70. 70. Ehret PJ, Ghaidarov TM, LaBrie JW. Can you say no? Examining the relationship between drinking refusal self-efficacy and protective behavioral strategy use on alcohol outcomes. Addict Behav. 2013;38(4):1898–904. pmid:23380495
  71. 71. Lannoy S, Mange J, Leconte P, Ritz L, Gierski F, Maurage P, et al. Distinct psychological profiles among college students with substance use: a cluster analytic approach. Addict Behav. 2020;109:106477. pmid:32485549
  72. 72. Mauduy M, Mauny N, Mange J. Tobacco dependence among french university students: a cluster analytic approach to identifying distinct psychological profiles of smokers. J Drug Issues. 2022;53(2):226–46.
  73. 73. Tavolacci MP, Ladner J, Grigioni S, Richard L, Villet H, Dechelotte P. Prevalence and association of perceived stress, substance use and behavioral addictions: a cross-sectional study among university students in France, 2009-2011. BMC Public Health. 2013;13:724. pmid:23919651
  74. 74. Cooke R, Beccaria F, Demant J, Fernandes-Jesus M, Fleig L, Negreiros J, et al. Patterns of alcohol consumption and alcohol-related harm among European university students. Eur J Public Health. 2019;29(6):1125–9. pmid:31079149
  75. 75. De Coninck D, Frissen T, Matthijs K, d’Haenens L, Lits G, Champagne-Poirier O, et al. Beliefs in conspiracy theories and misinformation about COVID-19: comparative perspectives on the role of anxiety, depression and exposure to and trust in information sources. Front Psychol. 2021;12:646394. pmid:33935904
  76. 76. Zunhammer M, Eberle H, Eichhammer P, Busch V. Somatic symptoms evoked by exam stress in university students: the role of alexithymia, neuroticism, anxiety and depression. PLoS One. 2013;8(12):e84911. pmid:24367700
  77. 77. Gijón Puerta J, Galván Malagón MC, Khaled Gijón M, Lizarte Simón EJ. Levels of stress, anxiety, and depression in university students from Spain and Costa Rica during periods of confinement and virtual learning. Educ Sci. 2022;12(10):660.
  78. 78. Mikolajczyk RT, Maxwell AE, Naydenova V, Meier S, El Ansari W. Depressive symptoms and perceived burdens related to being a student: survey in three European countries. Clin Pract Epidemiol Ment Health. 2008;4:19. pmid:18598340
  79. 79. Gadermann AM, Guhn M, Zumbo BD. Estimating ordinal reliability for likert-type and ordinal item response data: a conceptual, empirical, and practical guide. Pract Assess Res Eval. 2012;17(3):1–13.
  80. 80. Townshend JM, Duka T. Patterns of alcohol drinking in a population of young social drinkers: a comparison of questionnaire and diary measures. Alcohol Alcohol. 2002;37(2):187–92. pmid:11912076
  81. 81. Townshend JM, Duka T. Binge drinking, cognitive performance and mood in a population of young social drinkers. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2005;29(3):317–25. pmid:15770105
  82. 82. Callero PL. Role-identity salience. Soc Psychol Q. 1985;48(3):203.
  83. 83. Kuntsche E, Kuntsche S. Development and validation of the Drinking Motive Questionnaire Revised Short Form (DMQ-R SF). J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2009;38(6):899–908. pmid:20183672
  84. 84. Billieux J, Rochat L, Ceschi G, Carré A, Offerlin-Meyer I, Defeldre A-C, et al. Validation of a short French version of the UPPS-P impulsive behavior scale. Compr Psychiatry. 2012;53(5):609–15. pmid:22036009
  85. 85. Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene R, Vagg PR, Jacobs GA. Manual for the stait-trait anxiety inventory. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press; 1983. https://doi.org/10.1037/t06496-000
  86. 86. Beck AT, Steer RA, Carbin MG. Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory: twenty-five years of evaluation. Clin Psychol Rev. 1988;8(1):77–100.
  87. 87. de Grâce G-R, Joshi P, Pelletier R. L’Échelle de solitude de l’Université Laval (ÉSUL): validation canadienne-française du UCLA Loneliness Scale. Can J Behav Sci. 1993;25(1):12–27.
  88. 88. Miller BE, Miller MN, Verhegge R, Linville HH, Pumariega AJ. Alcohol misuse among college athletes: self-medication for psychiatric symptoms? J Drug Educ. 2002;32(1):41–52. pmid:12096556
  89. 89. Pek J, Wong O, Wong ACM. How to address non-normality: a taxonomy of approaches, reviewed, and illustrated. Front Psychol. 2018;9:2104. pmid:30459683
  90. 90. Rousselet GA, Pernet CR, Wilcox RR. The percentile bootstrap: a primer with step-by-step instructions in R. Adv Methods Pract Psychol Sci. 2021;4(1).
  91. 91. Yzerbyt V, Muller D, Batailler C, Judd CM. New recommendations for testing indirect effects in mediational models: the need to report and test component paths. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2018;115(6):929–43. pmid:30550319
  92. 92. Korkmaz S, Goksuluk D, Zararsiz G. MVN: an R package for assessing multivariate normality. R J. 2014;6(2):151–62.
  93. 93. Rosseel Y. lavaan: An R package for structural equation. J Stat Softw. 2012;48(2).
  94. 94. Malmberg M, Kleinjan M, Vermulst AA, Overbeek G, Monshouwer K, Lammers J, et al. Do substance use risk personality dimensions predict the onset of substance use in early adolescence? A variable- and person-centered approach. J Youth Adolesc. 2012;41(11):1512–25. pmid:22623315
  95. 95. Cox WM, Klinger E, Fadardi JS. The motivational basis of cognitive determinants of addictive behaviors. Addict Behav. 2015;44:16–22. pmid:25499579
  96. 96. Mezquita L, Stewart SH, Ibáñez MI, Ruipérez MA, Villa H, Moya J, et al. Drinking motives in clinical and general populations. Eur Addict Res. 2011;17(5):250–61. pmid:21691118
  97. 97. West R, Brown J. Theory of addiction. West Sussex, UK: John Wiley and Sons; 2013.
  98. 98. Mackinnon SP, Kehayes I-LL, Clark R, Sherry SB, Stewart SH. Testing the four-factor model of personality vulnerability to alcohol misuse: a three-wave, one-year longitudinal study. Psychol Addict Behav. 2014;28(4):1000–12. pmid:25134058
  99. 99. Lee CM, Geisner IM, Patrick ME, Neighbors C. The social norms of alcohol-related negative consequences. Psychol Addict Behav. 2010;24(2):342–8. pmid:20565160
  100. 100. Wood MD, Nagoshi CT, Dennis DA. Alcohol norms and expectations as predictors of alcohol use and problems in a college student sample. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse. 1992;18(4):461–76. pmid:1449125
  101. 101. DeJong W. Social norms marketing campaigns to reduce campus alcohol problems. Health Commun. 2010;25(6–7):615–6. pmid:20845167
  102. 102. Neighbors C, Lee CM, Lewis MA, Fossos N, Larimer ME. Are social norms the best predictor of outcomes among heavy-drinking college students? J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2007;68(4):556–65. pmid:17568961
  103. 103. Creswell KG. Drinking together and drinking alone: a social-contextual framework for examining risk for alcohol use disorder. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2021;30(1):19–25. pmid:35291310
  104. 104. Creswell KG, Chung T, Wright AGC, Clark DB, Black JJ, Martin CS. Personality, negative affect coping, and drinking alone: a structural equation modeling approach to examine correlates of adolescent solitary drinking. Addiction. 2015;110(5):775–83. pmid:25664806
  105. 105. Skrzynski C, Creswell KG, Bachrach RL, Chung T. Social discomfort moderates the relationship between drinking in response to negative affect and solitary drinking in underage drinkers. Addict Behav. 2018;78:124–30. pmid:29154151
  106. 106. Labhart F, Kuntsche E, Wicki M, Gmel G. Reciprocal influences of drinking motives on alcohol use and related consequences: a full cross-lagged panel study among young adult men. Behav Med. 2017;43(4):277–84. pmid:26959722
  107. 107. Vallentin-Holbech L, Rasmussen BM, Stock C. Effects of the social norms intervention The GOOD Life on norm perceptions, binge drinking and alcohol-related harms: a cluster-randomised controlled trial. Prev Med Rep. 2018;12:304–11. pmid:30406009
  108. 108. Mauduy M, Priolo D, Margas N, Sénémeaud C. Does the first step of the induced-hypocrisy paradigm really matter? An initial investigation using a meta-analytic approach. Soc Psychol Pers Sci. 2023;15(5):529–39.
  109. 109. Kang SK, Bodenhausen GV. Multiple identities in social perception and interaction: challenges and opportunities. Annu Rev Psychol. 2015;66:547–74. pmid:25061671
  110. 110. Mollen S, Engelen S, Kessels LTE, van den Putte B. Short and sweet: the persuasive effects of message framing and temporal context in antismoking warning labels. J Health Commun. 2017;22(1):20–8. pmid:27997285
  111. 111. Gallagher KM, Updegraff JA. Health message framing effects on attitudes, intentions, and behavior: a meta-analytic review. Ann Behav Med. 2012;43(1):101–16. pmid:21993844
  112. 112. Donohew L, Sypher HE, Bukoski WJ. Persuasive communication and drug abuse prevention. Routledge; 2012.
  113. 113. Rupp CI, Kemmler G, Kurz M, Hinterhuber H, Fleischhacker WW. Cognitive remediation therapy during treatment for alcohol dependence. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2012;73(4):625–34. pmid:22630801
  114. 114. Hofmann SG, Gómez AF. Mindfulness-based interventions for anxiety and depression. Psychiatr Clin North Am. 2017;40(4):739–49. pmid:29080597
  115. 115. Jones KA, Chryssanthakis A, Groom MJ. Impulsivity and drinking motives predict problem behaviours relating to alcohol use in university students. Addict Behav. 2014;39(1):289–96. pmid:24176824