Abstract
This study aimed to evaluate the effect of auditory stimuli on mares and geldings during social isolation and movement restriction, and their ability to promote the reduction of stress responses. The research was conducted with eight Pantaneiro horses, divided into Experiment I (4 mares) and Experiment II (4 geldings), both experiments were executed equally using a 4x4 Latin square design with experimental (classical, country and new age) and control (no music) treatments. Physiological parameters (heart rate, heart rate variability, respiratory rate, ocular temperature by infrared thermography, and surface temperature by infrared thermometer) and facial expressions (eye closure (AU143), blink (AU145), half blink (AU47), inner brow raiser (AU101), eye white increase (AD1), tongue show (AD19), nostril dilator (AD38), chewing (AD81), ears forward (EAD101), and ear rotator (EAD104)) were assessed throughout the 24 minutes that the animals remained in the restraining stock. In Experiment I, there was a reduction (p < 0.05) in heart rate during classical and country treatments and an increase (p < 0.05) in auricular and body temperatures during the classical treatment. Additionally, the frequencies of expressions of nostril dilator (AD38), ear rotator (EAD104), and half blink (AU47) were lower (p < 0.05). In Experiment II, there was an increase (p < 0.05) in ocular temperature during the country and control treatments, and a higher frequency (p < 0.05) of ears forward (EAD101) during the country treatment. Exposure to Beethoven’s 9th Symphony (classical genre) and Hank Williams Jr.’s “Ramblin’ In My Shoes” (country genre) reduced stress in mares. Geldings showed less pronounced responses to music genres, indicating a possible preference for Janet Marlow’s “Horsing Around” (new age genre). Music can be used as auditory enrichment for horses in the specific context of this study.
Figures
Citation: da Fé VCS, dos Santos VMO, de Lima ACB, Hernandes MSP, Caldara FR, Gomes MDNB (2025) Auditory enrichment on facial and physiological responses of Pantaneiro geldings and mares under short-term stress. PLoS One 20(5): e0323649. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0323649
Editor: Julio Cesar de Souza, Universidade Federal de Mato Grosso do Sul, BRAZIL
Received: October 17, 2024; Accepted: April 12, 2025; Published: May 20, 2025
Copyright: © 2025 da Fé et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Data Availability: The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the Postgraduate Program in Animal Science at the Federal University of Mato Grosso do Sul upon request (contact e-mail: cianimal.famez@ufms.br).
Funding: This study was financed in part by the Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior - Brazil (CAPES) - Finance Code 001 [https://www.gov.br/capes/pt-br], and by the Fundação de Apoio ao Desenvolvimento do Ensino, Ciência e Tecnologia do Estado de Mato Grosso do Sul (FUNDECT) [https://www.fundect.ms.gov.br/], through the scholarships granted.
Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Introduction
The Pantaneiro horse originated from Iberian horses brought to Brazil during the colonial period [1]. Once introduced into the Pantanal, these horses underwent natural selection over the years, developing characteristics that form an ecotype well-adapted to the environmental conditions of this biome, characterized by high temperatures, and periods of flooding and drought [2,3]. The breed is highly relevant to the local population, serving as an important means of transportation, particularly in regions with difficult access. Additionally, the notable adaptive traits of the Pantaneiro horse confer unique genetic value and significant utility in cattle management, which is the main economic activity in the region [4]. Currently, there is a growing interest in the husbandry of this native breed.
Equines, like other domesticated species, have basic physiological and behavioral needs that require attention. When the basic needs of the species are not met, welfare is compromised [5]. Thus, the domestic environment can be extremely challenging, particularly for animals kept under suboptimal conditions, as is the case in intensive farming systems where the expression of natural behavior is restricted [6].
It is common for stabled horses to be kept in social isolation. However, for a species that naturally lives in herds, close contact with conspecifics is ideal due to evolutionary pressures and their vulnerability as prey [7]. In this context, the restriction of movement resulting from the physical limitations imposed on animals raised in intensive systems is extremely detrimental, considering that free-ranging horses travel several kilometers daily [8]. In addition to social isolation and movement restriction, the routine management performed using restraining stocks, whether for medical intervention, sanitary control, or reproductive purposes, can induce stress [9], and consequently, have a negative impact on welfare.
In this scenario, auditory environmental enrichment emerges as a means to enhance the quality of life and ensure the welfare of equines by reproducing sound for animals that do not have access to open environments or have been temporarily isolated. It can also be used when introducing an animal to an unfamiliar enclosure or area, as a way to minimize impact and promote relaxation and calmness [10].
Research has been conducted to evaluate the effect of music on the physiological and behavioral parameters of equines. In a study by [11], new age music was played for geriatric horses for 28 days, resulting in a significant relaxation of these animals. Additionally, it was found that instrumental guitar music could positively influence Arabian racehorses when played for 5 hours daily [12]. Another study observed that the beneficial effects of music were more pronounced after 3 hours of daily exposure [13].
In the literature, most studies evaluate the impact of music over prolonged periods. Aiming to develop new application strategies, this study investigated the ability of different musical genres, previously tested and reported for equines, to promote the reduction of short-term stress responses, resulting from restraint handling in stocks (movement restriction) and social isolation.
To assess these responses, non-invasive indicators were used to express changes in physiological parameters [14]. Additionally, to provide a novel complement, facial expressions were included to analyze animals under handling stress and exposure to music. A range of facial expressions has been associated with specific emotional states, such as stress, and serves as a tool for evaluating welfare [15]. Few studies have been conducted on horses to evaluate facial expressions during potentially stressful handling situations, such as clipping [16], transportation [17], food restriction [18], and social isolation [17].
Therefore, to ensure the safe and effective use of auditory enrichment, it is necessary to conduct studies with a variety of musical genres in a range of contexts related to the typical challenges faced by horses in domestic environments. Thus, this study aims to evaluate whether auditory enrichment can attenuate short-term stress responses, resulting in a reduction of stress indicators. It also seeks to identify differences in responses between geldings and mares when exposed to different musical genres and to determine whether classical, new age and country music can promote relaxation.
Materials and methods
Ethics in animal research
All procedures conducted in these studies were approved by the Animal Ethics and Use Committee (CEUA) of the Federal University of Mato Grosso do Sul (UFMS, Campo Grande, Brazil) under protocol number 1.222/2022.
Location
Experiments I and II were conducted with a one-week interval in 2023, both at the farm of the Faculty of Veterinary Medicine and Animal Science located in Terenos, MS, Central-West region of Brazil (20° 26′ 18″ S, 54° 51′ 24″ W), which has an Aw climate (tropical savanna with dry winters), according to Köppen’s classification [19].
Microclimatic parameters
During the evaluations, meteorological variables were collected at ten-minute intervals: dry bulb temperature (DBT, °C), wet bulb temperature (WBT, °C), black globe temperature (BGT, °C), dew point temperature (DPT, °C), relative humidity (RH%), and wind speed (WS, m/s).
For the measurements, digital thermo-hygrometers (AK172®; AKSO, São Leopoldo, RS, Brazil) were used, and placed in meteorological shelters. For the black globe temperature, the same model of thermo-hygrometer was encapsulated in PVC plastic balls (0.15 cm in diameter) and painted externally with matte black paint according to the method proposed by [20]. The equipment was placed in two locations: in full sunlight and in full shade within the installation where the animals were kept during the tests. The devices were installed 1.50 meters above the ground surface, considering the variation in shadow projection and zenithal angle during the period of insolation.
Based on the collected variables, the following indices were calculated: radiant heat load (W/m2), according to the equation proposed by [21]; wet bulb globe temperature (WBGT, °C), according to the equation described by [22]; and thermal comfort index (TCI), as proposed by [23]. This allowed for the characterization of the environment and the thermal condition to which the animals were exposed. The mean values and standard deviations obtained in Experiments I and II were, respectively: dry bulb temperature (29 ± 1.2; 25 ± 2.8), relative humidity (70 ± 3.8; 84 ± 7.8), radiant heat load (468 ± 5.7; 422 ± 11.9), WBGT (26 ± 1.0; 22 ± 2.0) and TCI (155 ± 1.5; 162 ± 2.8).
The experimental period took place during the summer. The recorded values for air temperature, relative humidity, and radiant heat load align with the expected conditions for the region during this season, as the Aw climate is characterized by rainy summers and high temperatures [19].
The WBGT has been used to identify the risk of thermal stress in horses [24,25]. The average values obtained were < 28 °C, indicating a low risk of thermal stress [25,26]. Although the TCI exceeded 130, a value considered thermally stressful [23], the animals did not exhibit typical signs of thermal discomfort. The Pantaneiro horse, a Brazilian breed developed in the Pantanal region, has adapted over centuries to the adverse conditions of flooding and drought, as well as to the high temperatures of the region [27–29]. The environmental conditions present in the study are typical and well-tolerated within the breed’s adaptive scenario.
Animals and housing
For Experiment I, four Pantaneiro mares were selected, with an average age of 5 ± 0.9 years and an average weight of 367.5 ± 23.6 kg. Subsequently, in Experiment II, four Pantaneiro geldings were selected, with an average age of 5 ± 1.8 years and an average weight of 388.7 ± 5.4 kg.
The animals belong to the Federal University of Mato Grosso do Sul and are maintained for research purposes. From birth, they have been kept in social groups housed in paddocks with ad libitum access to pasture (Panicum maximum cv. Tamani), mineral salt, and water, and they remained in these conditions during the experimental period when not being evaluated.
Experimental design
The design used in both experiments was a 4x4 Latin square with experimental (musical genre) and control (no music) treatments. Four animals were individually assessed each day in the morning (7.30–10.30 a.m.). Each horse received all treatments randomly (one treatment per day) at the same time, thereby allowing each animal to serve as its own control.
The genres and corresponding musical styles chosen for auditory stimulation were evaluated in previous studies for equines, namely: Classical (Beethoven – 9th Symphony [30,31]), Country (Hank Williams Jr – Ramblin’ In My Shoes [32]), and New Age (Janet Marlow – Horsing Around [11,33]). The composer Janet Marlow, a specialist in music for animals, developed the music ‘Horsing Around’ specifically adjusted for equine hearing (My Pet Speaker, Pet Acoustics Inc).
The spectrogram (Fig 1) is a tool for describing complex acoustic signals, allowing visualization of sound recordings in three dimensions: frequency (Hz), time (s), and amplitude (dB). Amplitude is represented by the intensity of color, with warmer or darker colors reflecting higher amplitudes [34]. This enables the observation of how the frequency spectrum of a piece of music changes over time, where higher frequencies characterize higher-pitched sounds and lower frequencies characterize lower-pitched sounds [35–37].
(a) Classical (Beethoven - 9th Symphony); (b) Country (Hank Williams Jr - Ramblin’ In My Shoes); (c) New Age (Janet Marlow - Horsing Around) (created by A.C. Bini de Lima using BORIS software resources).
In the classical song ‘9th Symphony’, a maximum frequency of 15,000 Hz is noted with peaks at lower frequencies, while in the country song ‘Ramblin in my shoes’ the maximum frequency is around 16,000 Hz and continues throughout time. The new age song ‘Horsing around’ has a maximum frequency of 20,000 Hz with oscillations over time. Through the spectrograms and the acoustic signals described, it is observed that the songs of the classical and country genres present low-er-pitched sounds, and the new age genre music has higher-pitched sounds.
Experimental protocol
Before the experimental period began, all animals underwent a habituation phase to get accustomed to the collection site, handling procedures, and evaluators. Over the course of one week, the animals were introduced to the restraining stock, where they remained for three minutes and were exposed to the equipment to be used throughout the experiment. No music was played during the habituation phase.
During the experimental period, to ensure social isolation and restriction of movement, each individual remained in a restraining stock (2.10m x 0.88m x 1.00m) located in a covered area with partially enclosed sides, preventing any contact with conspecifics. Furthermore, the area was closed off, restricting the circulation of people and mitigating acoustic environmental effects during the collection.
After the collection of each animal, the restraining stock was cleaned to ensure hygiene and prevent olfactory contact.
For the administration of the experimental treatment, a JBL GO3 was positioned in front of the animal at a height of 0.70 m above the ground, two meters away, for 24 minutes for auditory stimulation. All music tracks were standardized in amplitude using the Decibel Meter App (Splend Apps) to ensure they reached 60–70 dB at the center of the experimental setup, a sound level corresponding to natural conditions [30,31,38] (Fig 2). During this period, the evaluator maintained a neutral facial expression to avoid potential influence on the animal’s behavior.
The physiological parameters and facial expressions were assessed during the auditory stimulus presentation. The following indicators were collected at specific time points identified as P1, P2, P3, and P4: ocular temperature by infrared thermography (OTT), surface temperature by infrared thermometer (STT), and respiratory rate (RR). Facial expressions (FE) and heart rate and heart rate variability (HR/HRV) were collected continuously but assessed only at five-minute intervals identified as I1, I2, and I3 (Fig 3).
OTT = ocular temperature by infrared thermography; STT = surface temperature by infrared thermometer; RR = respiratory rate; FE = facial expressions; HR = heart rate; HRV = heart rate variability (created by V.C.S. da Fé using Canva resources).
Physiological parameters
Heart Rate (HR) and Heart Rate Variability (HRV).
Heart rate and heart rate variability were measured using the Polar H10 heart rate transmitter (Polar Electro Oy, Kempele, Finland). The Polar H10 was secured to an elastic belt and positioned on the thoracic region between the 4th and 5th intercostal spaces on the left side of the chest. The belt was moistened with water and adjusted on the animals 10 minutes before data collection. To enhance the transmission of electrical signals from the body to the electrodes, the hair was also cleaned with water.
Subsequently, the collected data were exported using the Elite HRV app (Elite HRV, Asheville, NC, USA), and data analysis was performed using Kubios HRV Standard software, version 3.5.0 (Kubios Oy, Kuopio, Finland). For analysis, a mean artifact correction was applied to reduce error across the entire sample set, following the methodology of [39], which allows for a correction of 15% or less. Out of the 32 samples collected, only 2 had values greater than 5%, specifically 5.29% and 8.06%. Three five-minute intervals were then selected for the analysis of HRV variables and mean HR. The following frequency domain HRV variables were selected: low-frequency power (LF; nu), high-frequency power (HF; nu), and low-frequency to high-frequency ratio (LF/HF).
Respiratory Rate (RR).
The respiratory rate, measured in movements per minute (mpm), was determined by counting flank movements over a period of 30 seconds. The value obtained was then multiplied by 2 to calculate the respiratory rate per minute.
Ocular Temperature by Infrared Thermography (OTT).
Thermographic images of the eye were collected to measure the superficial temperature in the medial canthus region, as proposed by [14]. Images were captured from the left side of the animal at a 90° angle to the sagittal plane, directed towards the eye at a distance of 0.5 meters, using an infrared thermographic camera (S60, Caterpillar FLIR, Vernon Hills, IL, USA). The camera’s emissivity was set to 0.98, a value corresponding to that used for tissue surfaces. The FLIR Tools software, version 6.4.18039.1003 (FLIR Systems Inc, Oregon, USA), was used to analyze the images.
Surface Temperature by Infrared Thermometer (STT).
The surface temperature was measured using a portable digital infrared thermometer (Mult Temp Portable, Incoterm, Porto Alegre, RS, Brazil). Measurements were taken at six points on the left side of the animals (Fig 4), as follows:
- Auricular: Central point of the ear cavity on the left ear of the animal [40];
- Neck Surface: Midpoint of the line connecting the most caudal point of the Atlas bone with the most prominent point of the scapular crest. This region of the neck has a rich blood supply, and this methodology was based on the studies by [41];
- Withers Surface: Located on the midline, an odd region situated cranially on the upper dorsal part of the trunk [42];
- Back Surface: Midpoint of the line connecting the most caudal point of the scapular cartilage with the iliac tuberosity. This point shows considerable thermal variability and is considered representative of the average body surface, according to the methodology adopted by [43];
- Croup Surface: Midpoint of the line connecting the iliac tuberosity with the tail insertion, following the methodology proposed by [43];
- Tail Insertion Surface: Unique point at the junction of the sacral and caudal vertebrae, projection of the coccygeal artery 10 cm from the start of the tail on the ventral portion [43].
The infrared thermometer was aimed at the animal’s coat from a distance of 1 meter, except for the auricular and tail insertion measurements, which were collected from a distance of 0.5 meters. For assessment, auricular temperature and body surface temperature (obtained from the average of all collected points excluding the auricular point) were selected.
Facial Expressions (FE)
During the period the animal was restrained in the stock, video recordings were made using a Canon EOS SL3 digital camera (Canon Inc., São Paulo, SP, Brazil). The resolution was set to 1080p at 30 fps, and the videos were exported in mp4 format. The camera was positioned 1.5 meters away at a 45° angle relative to the medial plane of the horse.
EquiFACS, as described by [15], was used for facial expression assessment. For this study, four Action Units (AUs), seven Action Descriptors (ADs), and one Visibility Code (VC) were selected, as outlined in Table 1. These codes correspond to facial expressions that have been shown to be relevant for assessing horses under stress conditions in previous studies [16–18].
The designated AUs represent the contraction of a specific facial muscle (or muscle group) and the resulting facial movements [44]. The ADs are also used for facial movements, but these are general movements where the underlying muscle cannot be identified, or they result from a different muscle group [15].
All videos were reviewed by a single EquiFACS-certified coder using BORIS software, version 6.0.6 (Friard and Gamba, University of Turin, Turin, Italy), with inter-observer agreement >70% compared to experienced coders and 95% intra-observer agreement. Intraclass correlation coefficients (ICCs) ranged from 0.99 to 1.00, demonstrating high concordance. Coding was performed on 5-minute clips obtained during the animals’ time in the stock (I1, I2, and I3).
Continuous focal sampling was used to record the facial expressions of each horse. The videos were initially observed at normal speed and then reviewed at least three more times in slow motion or frame-by-frame. The frequency per minute of each of the proposed codes was assessed, except for VC73, which was measured in seconds. VC73 was used to indicate how long the entire face was not visible for coding.
This approach allowed for monitoring the duration for which the face was in a visible position for coding, or not, during each 5-minute interval. To calculate the frequency per minute of AUs and ADs, only the time in which the face was in a visible position for coding was considered:
Statistical description
All statistical analyses were performed using R software with the RStudio integrated development environment (Version 4.1.0 (2021-06-29), RStudio, Inc.). The functions and packages utilized are presented in the format ‘package::function’ corresponding to the R programming language. A significance level of 5% was considered for all tests.
Initially, Pearson’s correlation coefficient (Hmisc::rcorr) was estimated among variables derived from heart rate (mean heart rate, LF, HF, and LF/HF). Of those highly correlated, only one (LF/HF) was selected for use in the inferential stage (modeling), based on its lower variance and greater biological relevance to the study’s aim.
Subsequently, an inferential analysis was conducted. To identify differences between treatments (classical vs country vs control vs new age) and over time (P1 vs P2 vs P3 vs P4 or I1 vs I2 vs I3), multilevel linear models (lme4::lmer) were fitted for response variables (LF/HF, respiratory rate, auricular temperature, ocular temperature, body temperature, inner brow raiser - AU101, nostril dilator - AD38, ears forward - EAD101, ear rotator - EAD104, chewing - AD81, blink - AU145, and half blink - AU47) where model residuals adhered to normality as assessed by the Cramer-Von Mises test (nortest::cvm.test). Multiple comparisons in the post-hoc test were conducted using Tukey’s procedure (lsmeans::lsmeans and multcomp::cld).
The interaction of collection points or intervals with treatments and the order were used as fixed effects in the modeling, while horses were used as random effects to control individual variation.
Mean heart rate, eye white increase (AD1), tongue show (AD19), and eye closure (AU143) did not meet the normality assumption even after transformations, preventing their modeling. Therefore, comparisons between each treatment at the same interval and comparisons over time for each treatment were conducted using the Friedman test (stats::friedman.test and PMCMR-plus::frdAllPairsNemenyiTest). Results were illustrated with box plots (ggplot2::ggplot and ggplot2::geom_boxplot).
Subsequently, an exploratory analysis was conducted. To analyze the multivariate dynamics between facial expressions, principal component analysis (PCA) was performed based on a correlation matrix (stats::princomp). The optimal number of principal components (PCs) to retain in PCA was determined by Horn’s parallel analysis (‘psych::fa.parallel’). To establish associations between each variable and the PCs, loadings greater than 0.40 or less than -0.40 were used as the criterion. The results were illustrated with two biplots, where observations were color-coded according to treatments for qualitative (visual) assessment of their distribution.
To ensure reliability in the coding of facial expressions, the intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) was calculated (irr::icc()) using data collected from ten randomly selected 5-minute videos of the study animals. The experimental design was set as “two-way” and “agreement,” resulting in the concordance between multiple measurements taken by the same observer on different occasions.
Results
Experiment I
The mean heart rate of the mares decreased (p < 0.05) across intervals (I1, I2, I3) for both classical and country music treatments (Table 2). The LF/HF ratio did not differ (p > 0.05) between treatments or intervals.
Body and auricular temperatures differed (p < 0.05) between time points (P1 and P4) in the classical treatment (Table 3). Respiratory rate and ocular temperature did not differ (p > 0.05) between treatments or time points.
The frequency of facial action descriptors nostril dilation (AD38) and ear rotator (EAD104) in the second interval (I2) was higher (p < 0.05) for the new age treatment and lower for the classical treatment (Table 4; Fig 5). In the third interval (I3), the frequency of the ear rotator (EAD104) expression was higher (p < 0.05) for the new age treatment and lower for the country treatment.
Different uppercase letters indicate significant statistical differences from post-hoc tests between treatments within the same interval (p < 0.05), where A > B; the black diamond represents the mean; each colored circle, corresponding to the respective treatment colors, represents an individual animal.
The frequency of the facial action unit half blink (AU47) during interval three (I3) was higher (p < 0.05) for the control treatment, and lower for the classical treatment. The new age and country treatments did not differ from each other.
Horn’s parallel analysis indicated the retention of the first and second principal components (PCs) out of a total of 5 PCs generated by the principal component analysis (PCA), thus only these were analyzed. The PCA loadings represent the level of association between a variable and a particular PC, with loadings further from zero indicating a higher level of positive (1.00) or negative (-1.00) association. Therefore, a set of variables positively associated with a PC exhibits similar dynamics, meaning they all increase or decrease concurrently.
Associations were determined using a cut-off loading value of 0.40, either positive or negative. PC1 alone accounted for the largest portion (31.4%) of the total variance in the data. The variables ears forward (EAD101), ear rotator (EAD104), half blink (AU47), eye white increase (AD1), nostril dilator (AD38), and chewing (AD81) showed positive associations (loading values > 0.40) with PC1 (Table 5). Thus, the increase or decrease in these facial expressions occurred concurrently across treatments. Nostril dilator (AD38) exhibited the loading value furthest from zero on PC1, indicating it as the facial expression with the greatest variation (importance) on PC1. Blink (AU145), tongue show (AD19), and eye closure (AU143) showed a positive association (loading values > 0.40) with PC2. In contrast, the inner brow raiser (AU101) was the only facial expression that exhibited a negative association with PC2.
Fig 6 separates the evaluations of mares across classical, control, country, and new age treatments, represented by different colors. One of the features of this figure is the centroid (larger circle), which indicates the center of mass derived from the polygon (geometric shape) formed by interpolating the evaluations (smaller circles) of the same color. Visual inspection suggests that the centroids for the classical and country treatments are distinct from those for the new age and control treatments, indicating that facial expressions were able to differentiate between the treatments.
Moreover, the centroids for the new age and control treatments are located in the lower right quadrant along with the vectors for ear rotator (EAD104), inner brow raiser (AU101), eye white increase (AD1), and nostril dilator (AD38). In contrast, the centroids for the classical and country treatments are positioned in the upper left quadrant along with the vectors for eye closure (AU143) and blink (AU145), suggesting a higher occurrence of these facial expressions in the respective treatments. These results explore a possible multifaceted dynamic of facial expressions that were able to distinguish treatments in the previous inferential analyses (modeling).
Experiment II
The average heart rate (bpm) and LF/HF ratio in geldings did not differ (p > 0.05) between treatments or across intervals (Table 6).
The respiratory rate at the first time point (P1) increased (p < 0.05), differing between the control and new age treatments. Ocular temperature increased (p < 0.05) between time points (P1 and P3) in the control and country treatments (Table 7).
The frequency of the action descriptor ears forward (EAD101) in interval three (I3) was higher (p < 0.05) for the country treatment and lower for the new age treatment (Table 8; Fig 7). The action unit eye closure (AU143) was not selected due to its low frequency.
Different uppercase letters indicate statistically significant differences from the post-hoc test between treatments within the same interval (p < 0.05), with A > B; the black diamond represents the mean; each colored circle corresponds to an animal, with colors representing the respective treatments.
Horn’s parallel analysis indicated the retention of the first and second principal components (PCs) out of a total of 5 PCs generated by principal component analysis (PCA); thus, only these two components were analyzed.
The PC1 alone accounted for the largest portion (28.8%) of the total data variation, with ears forward (EAD101), ear rotator (EAD104), inner brow raiser (AU101), half blink (AU47), and chewing (AD81) showing a positive association (loading values >0.40) with PC1 (Table 9).
Consequently, the decrease or increase in these facial expressions occurred concurrently across treatments. Ears forward (EAD101) exhibited the most extreme loading value on PC1, which can be interpreted as the facial expression with the greatest variation (importance) on PC1. Blink (AU145) and tongue show (AD19) showed a positive association (loading values >0.40) with PC2.
Fig 8 separates the evaluations of horses across the classical, control, country, and new age treatments using different colors. A visual inspection reveals that the centroids of the classical, country, and control treatments are distinct from the centroids of the new age treatment, indicating that facial expressions were able to differentiate the treatments.
Moreover, the centroids of the classical, country, and control treatments are located in the lower-right quadrant, overlapping and close to the center of the quadrants, along with the vectors for inner brow raiser (AU101), eye white increase (AD1), ears forward (EAD101), and half blink (AU47). In contrast, the centroid of the new age treatment is positioned in the upper-left quadrant, along with the vector for blink (AU145), suggesting a higher occurrence of these facial expressions in their respective treatments. These results explore the potential multiple dynamics of facial expressions that were capable of distinguishing treatments in previous inferential analyses (modeling).
Discussion
Heart rate (HR) and heart rate variability (HRV) have been extensively used to investigate the functioning of the autonomic nervous system (ANS), particularly the balance between vagal and sympathetic activity, in relation to coping strategies in horses.
The vagal branch of the ANS is associated with adaptive responsiveness to the environment [45], where individuals with higher parasympathetic activity tend to be more exploratory and adaptive to environmental demands.
In humans, the LF/HF ratio < 1.00 indicates a high level of relaxation of the body, characterizing parasympathetic activity on the heart [46], a fact also reported in a study by [47] who evaluated horses and found greater parasympathetic tone with values < 1.00. In Experiment I, the LF/HF ratio for mares presented values > 1.50 (ranging from 1.99 to 3.09), while in Experiment II, the ratio for geldings was > 1.00 (ranging from 1.06 to 2.98). Although there was no statistical difference between treatments, in both experiments, it was observed that the animals were under stress due to movement restriction from being restrained in the stock, with geldings showing greater resilience compared to mares. A study [48] found that sex may play an important role, with mares exhibiting a more pronounced stress reaction, showing higher values compared to geldings.
In investigating the reliability of heart rate variability (HRV) measures in horses [5], found that within a 4 m2 stall, where animals had the possibility of free, albeit limited or controlled, movements, parasympathetic control predominated. However, when restricted in a stock, activation of the sympathetic nervous system was observed, indicating that restriction of movement in the stock causes stress in the animals.
According to [29,49], the resting heart rate for adult horses, measured in environments with high temperatures (22°C to 36°C), can range from 28 to 40 beats per minute. In Experiment I, the heart rate of the mares was above the normal range for the equine species, showing values from 44 to 51 bpm. However, classical and country music were able to reduce the heart rate over time (p < 0.05), suggesting a calming effect.
A study conducted with women [50], indicated that the sympathetic nervous system is suppressed and the parasympathetic system is increased during exposure to classical music such as ‘Pachelbel’s Canon’ by the Jean-François Paillard Orchestra, suggesting a relaxing effect based on physiological parameters.
Contrary to the findings of [51], where country music induces agitation in various species due to its rhythm, the present study observed a relaxing effect. Additionally [52], conducted a study with humans and found that during auditory stimulation with heavy metal music, which is characterized by an agitated rhythm similar to country music, there was also a reduction in heart rate, indicating an acute decrease in autonomic cardiac regulation.
Consistent with the findings from Experiment I, musical genres such as country and classical positively affect the well-being of cattle, resulting in lower heart rates [53]. Additionally [54], reported that in everyday management situations for horses, classical music reduced the intensity of stress responses during short-duration transport or farrier treatments, suggesting that music may have practical implications.
In Experiment II, geldings showed heart rates ranging from 36 to 43 bpm, values close to the baseline range for the species, with no significant differences between treatments (p > 0.05). This contrasts with the results of [32], who exposed socially isolated male ponies to Hank Williams Jr.‘s country music and observed increased time spent feeding and a reduction in whinnying compared to the period without music.
The basal respiratory rate for horses in environments with high temperatures is between 10 and 20 breaths per minute [27,48]. In Experiments I and II, the results obtained were close to the established baseline range for the species, with values ranging from 19 to 28 and 16–25 breaths per minute, respectively. It is noteworthy that Pantaneiro horses are adapted and resilient to their environment, as reported by [28,29], who found that activities involving tourist rides and cattle handling in the Pantanal region did not significantly alter respiratory and heart rates before and after physical exertion.
The respiratory rate (RR) of geldings was lower at the initial time point (P1) of the control treatment, differing from the other treatments (p < 0.05). This effect may be related to the ‘novelty,’ meaning that the animals were exposed to unfamiliar auditory stimuli for the first time. However, although not significant, there was a trend towards an inversion in the responses where the RR of the control treatment increased numerically, while the RR of the musical genres decreased.
In Experiment I, mares exposed to classical music showed an increase in body temperature over time, suggesting a reduction in stress during restraint handling. A study with humans [49] also observed an increase in surface temperature of participants when listening to classical music, with a tendency for increased blood flow, indicating a potential calming effect.
Despite controversial responses regarding surface temperature and stress, the literature [55] supports this variable as an indicator of stress or well-being, where acute responses have been associated with patterns of vasoconstriction and vasodilation. In research conducted with healthy humans [56], found that stress led to changes in skin temperature, decreasing distally but increasing proximally, or remaining unchanged, indicating that there is no direct translation of this stress-induced hyperthermia paradigm.
The increase in the auricular temperature of mares exposed to classical music suggests relaxation or a lower level of stress. This result is consistent with reports from [56], which indicated that during a defensive/alarm response in horses, vasoconstriction can occur, leading to a subsequent decrease in auricular temperature. Similarly [57], observed a progressive reduction in the temperature of the auditory pavilion in rabbits subjected to a 15-minute immobilization procedure. Restraint is an action that induces fear and tension in animals regardless of species. Therefore, it can be inferred that the reduction in auricular temperature may be a plausible indicator of the potential calming effect of music on animals in stressful situations.
In Experiment II, the ocular temperature of the geldings was influenced (p < 0.05) by country music and the control (no music), showing a consistent increase over time, followed by a peak and subsequent decrease. This pattern of temperature elevation in response to a stressor is likely due to increased dilation of the ocular blood vessels and heightened visual attention [48].
Monitoring changes in ocular temperature has been used to assess equine responses to potentially stressful situations [58–61]. A decrease followed by an increase in temperature around the eyes (lacrimal caruncle) is associated with sympathetic [62] and parasympathetic [60,63] nervous system responses, respectively. In one study [64], observed an increase in ocular temperature in horses during potentially aversive handling, such as clipping, with temperature peaking after 10 minutes of the activity and decreasing immediately after the procedure. However, the ocular temperature did not return to baseline levels, possibly due to the metabolic consequences of the stress response.
To complement the physiological responses, the facial expressions of the animals were analyzed, focusing on those most indicative of stress, as evidenced by previous research on horses. In Experiment I, the facial expressions of nostril dilator (AD38), ear rotator (EAD104), and half blink (AU47) in mares exhibited significant treatment effects (p < 0.05). In Experiment II, with geldings, the treatment effect was significant only for the expression of ears forward (EAD101).
Nostril dilator (AD38) is a facial movement associated with deep breathing and sniffing, where the nostril diameter can vary depending on the animal’s physiological and psychological state [65]. Nostril dilator was observed in alert postures where the cardiorespiratory system is activated to prepare for potential flight [66], and it is detected in stressful situations [17]. Mares exhibited a higher frequency of this expression when exposed to new age music, concurrently with an increased frequency of ear rotator (EAD104).
In a study by [67], it was found that ear rotator (EAD104) is present in stressful situations resulting from social isolation. In Experiment I, the movement of the ear rotator, contrary to the direction from which the music was playing in front of the animal, suggests that during exposure to new age music, mares exhibited an aversion to the music. This expression occurred concurrently with an increased frequency of half blink (AU47).
Half blink (AU47) is characterized by a reduction in eye opening by the eyelids, without complete closure of the eyes. It is prevalent in horses in situations of pain [68], which can be considered a stressor [69]. In Experiment I, mares exhibited a higher frequency of half blink during the control treatment (no music), displaying a pattern similar to other stress-related facial expressions, suggesting that the animals were experiencing stress due to the restraint.
In contrast, blink (AU145) and eye closure (AU143) were more pronounced in mares during exposure to country and classical music, being the only expressions that exhibited behavior opposite to the others, suggesting an indication of relaxation. This might have contributed to the animals keeping their eyes closed for longer periods and/or more frequently, which is a requirement for coding both expressions, differentiated only by the speed of occurrence [15]. When analyzing the multiple dynamics of facial expressions in geldings, a similar trend was observed, with blink (AU145) showing behavior opposite to the others, being more evident during the new age treatment.
In Experiment II, geldings exhibited a higher frequency of ears forward (EAD101) during exposure to country music and a lower frequency when exposed to new age music. According to [70], the movement of ears forward occurs in horses under conditions of attention. This increase suggests that country music induced a state of alertness, with the animals keeping their ears directed forward for a larger portion of the time toward the auditory stimulus.
The results further indicate that in Experiment I, the lowest frequencies of facial expressions observed in mares—specifically nostril dilator (AD38), ear rotator (EAD104), and half blink (AU47)—occurred during exposure to classical and/or country music. These findings are consistent with the physiological parameters showing a reduction in average heart rate during classical and country music, and an increase in both auricular and body temperatures during classical music. This suggests a preference for classical and country music among the mares, which were able to reduce stress and provide a relaxing effect during restraint in the stocks. Conversely, the new age music, which had effects similar to the absence of music, was perceived as aversive.
This preference of mares for classical and country music genres can be explained by the frequency and tone characteristics of the music. As previously noted, the music used in the study had lower tones and frequencies around 15.000 Hz. The auditory sensitivity of horses is optimal between 1.000 Hz and 16.000 Hz [10]. A study by [71] aimed at identifying mare’s preferences for certain types of vocalizations found that they were more attracted to and showed greater attention to stallions with low-frequency vocalizations, indicating a potential biological preference.
The ‘Horsing Around’ music, of the new age genre, used in the experiments was specially produced for horses. However, the divergent results observed in Experiment I with the mares were similar to those found in another study by [72], which investigated the effect of music on stress reduction in dogs and also found no benefits with music specifically created for that species.
In contrast, in Experiment II, the facial expressions of the geldings suggest a possible preference for the new age music genre, corroborating a study that evaluated the same genre of music for geriatric horses and confirmed its positive effect on relaxation [11]. Classical and country music showed a similar effect to the control treatment. This is evidenced by the increase in ocular temperature observed during both the country music and control treatment, indicating a stress response. Thus, it can be inferred that the geldings reacted indifferently to the music, with no effective reduction in stress.
Notably, there is a difference between mares and geldings regarding preferences, including musical genres. Differences have also been observed in humans, where [73] suggested that psychophysiological responses to music may be influenced by hormonal state. In this context [74], indicated that men are more likely to experience less anxiety than women when exposed to a stressor, suggesting that they may become less anxious when the stressor is less imminent. This phenomenon is also observed between geldings and mares [75].
Similarly, this justification can be applied to the differences in skin temperature and autonomic cardiac modulation. [51] described that in a small sample of male volunteers, there was no influence of musical auditory stimulation with varying times and styles.
The results of this study underscore the complexity of stress responses in horses and the necessity for a personalized approach to animal welfare management. Factors such as the genre of music, the sex of the animals, and specific handling conditions should be carefully considered to maximize the benefits of auditory enrichment. Classical and country music proved promising in reducing stress in mares, while geldings may respond more positively to new age music. For practical application of these findings to improve the welfare of horses in controlled environments, it is essential to consider the genre of music, sex, breed, handling, and duration of exposure.
Conclusions
The ‘9th Symphony’ by Beethoven (classical genre) and ‘Ramblin’ In My Shoes’ by Hank Williams Jr. (country genre) promote stress reduction and a consequent relaxing effect in Pantaneiro mares during social isolation and movement restriction. In contrast, Pantaneiro geldings exhibited a less pronounced response, showing a possible preference for ‘Horsing Around’ by Janet Marlow (new age genre) under the same handling conditions.
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank the Postgraduate Program in Animal Science at the Federal University of Mato Grosso do Sul for the opportunity to carry out this work.
References
- 1.
Egito A, Mcmanus C, Albuquerque M, Sereno F, Santos A, Paiva S, et al. Caracterização genética. In: Santos AS, Salis SM, Comastri Filho JA, editors. Cavalo pantaneiro: rústico por natureza, vol. 10. Brasília DF: Embrapa SPI; 2016. p. 233–57.
- 2.
Santos SA. Por que conservar o cavalo pantaneiro? 2008.
- 3.
Santos A, Paiva S, Filho J, Ramos A, Juliano R, Nogueira M, et al. Histórico, vivências e desafios do núcleo de conservação do cavalo. 2020.
- 4.
Santos A, Mcmanus C, Paula Fd, Aníbal J, Filho C. Evolução demográfica do cavalo pantaneiro nos municípios de Mato Grosso e Mato Grosso do Sul. 2013.
- 5. Krueger K, Esch L, Farmer K, Marr I. Basic Needs in Horses?-A Literature Review. Animals (Basel). 2021;11(6):1798. pmid:34208615
- 6. Bini de Lima AC, Sebastião da Fé VC, Palermo Hernandes MS, Oliveira Dos Santos VM. Olfactory Stimulation as Environmental Enrichment for Domestic Horses-A Review. Animals (Basel). 2023;13(20):3180. pmid:37893904
- 7.
Mills DS, Ricci-Bonot C, Hall SS. Mental health issues in the horse. In: Mental health and well-being in animals. Wallingford, UK: CABI; 2020. p. 242–56. https://doi.org/10.1079/9781786393401.0242
- 8.
Beaver B. Equine behavioral medicine. Academic Press; 2019.
- 9. Vitale V, Balocchi R, Varanini M, Sgorbini M, Macerata A, Sighieri C, et al. The effects of restriction of movement on the reliability of heart rate variability measurements in the horse (Equus caballus). Journal of Veterinary Behavior. 2013;8(5):400–3.
- 10. Rørvang MV, Nielsen BL, McLean AN. Sensory Abilities of Horses and Their Importance for Equitation Science. Front Vet Sci. 2020;7:633. pmid:33033724
- 11. Wiśniewska M, Janczarek I, Wilk I, Wnuk-Pawlak E. Use of Music Therapy in Aiding the Relaxation of Geriatric Horses. J Equine Vet Sci. 2019;78:89–93. pmid:31203990
- 12. Stachurska A, Janczarek I, Wilk I, Kędzierski W. Does Music Influence Emotional State in Race Horses? Journal of Equine Veterinary Science. 2015;35(8):650–6.
- 13. Kędzierski W, Janczarek I, Stachurska A, Wilk I. Comparison of Effects of Different Relaxing Massage Frequencies and Different Music Hours on Reducing Stress Level in Race Horses. Journal of Equine Veterinary Science. 2017;53:100–7.
- 14. Kim S-M, Cho G-J. Validation of Eye Temperature Assessed Using Infrared Thermography as an Indicator of Welfare in Horses. Applied Sciences. 2021;11(16):7186.
- 15. Wathan J, Burrows AM, Waller BM, McComb K. EquiFACS: The Equine Facial Action Coding System. PLoS One. 2015;10(8):e0131738. pmid:26244573
- 16. Mott RO, Hawthorne SJ, McBride SD. Blink rate as a measure of stress and attention in the domestic horse (Equus caballus). Sci Rep. 2020;10(1):21409. pmid:33293559
- 17. Lundblad J, Rashid M, Rhodin M, Haubro Andersen P. Effect of transportation and social isolation on facial expressions of healthy horses. PLoS One. 2021;16(6):e0241532. pmid:34086704
- 18. Merkies K, Ready C, Farkas L, Hodder A. Eye Blink Rates and Eyelid Twitches as a Non-Invasive Measure of Stress in the Domestic Horse. Animals (Basel). 2019;9(8):562. pmid:31443315
- 19. Souza A de, Abreu MC, de Oliveira-Júnior JF, Aristone F, Fernandes WA, Aviv-Sharon E, et al. Climate Regionalization in Mato Grosso do Sul: a Combination of Hierarchical and Non-hierarchical Clustering Analyses Based on Precipitation and Temperature. Braz arch biol technol. 2022;65.
- 20. Souza C, Tinôco I, Baêta F, Ferreira W, Silva R. Avaliação de materiais alternativos para confecção do termômetro de globo. Ciência e Agrotecnologia. 2002;26:157–64.
- 21.
Esmay ML. Principles of animal environment. Westport: AVI Publisher; 1979.
- 22. Schroter RC, Marlin DJ, Jeffcott LB. Use of the Wet Bulb Globe Temperature (WBGT) index to quantify environmental heat loads during three-day-event competitions. Equine Vet J Suppl. 1996;(22):3–6. pmid:8894544
- 23. Jones S. Horseback riding in the dog days. Anim Sci e-News. 2009;2:3–4.
- 24. Kang H, Zsoldos RR, Sole-Guitart A, Narayan E, Cawdell-Smith AJ, Gaughan JB. Heat stress in horses: a literature review. Int J Biometeorol. 2023;67(6):957–73. pmid:37060454
- 25. Tantasavasdi C, Srisuwan W, Inprom N. Effect of opening on environmental conditions of a naturally ventilated stable in Thailand. Building and Environment. 2021;200:107984.
- 26. Purswell JL, Gates RS, Lawrence LM, Davis JD. Thermal Environment in a Four-Horse Slant-Load Trailer. Trans ASABE. 2010;200:1885–1894.
- 27. Santos AS, Paiva S, Comastri Filho JÁ, Ramos A, Juliano R, Nogueira M, et al. Histórico, vivências e desafios do núcleo de conservação do cavalo Pantaneiro. Rev Ciênc Agrár. 2020.
- 28. Senna D, Luiz MA, Padilha FGF, Fonseca ABM, Cassia VR, Ferreira AMR. Evaluation of physiological parameters of pantaneiro horses undergoing tourist ride/avaliacao dos parametros fisiologicos de equinos da raca pantaneira submetidos a cavalgada turistica/evaluacion de parametros fisiologicos de caballos pantaneiros sometidos a cabalgada turistica. Veterinaria e Zootecnia. 2017;24:140–4.
- 29. Silva LD, Santos AS, Silva RAS, McManus C, Petzold H. Adaptação do cavalo pantaneiro ao estresse da lida diária de gado no Pantanal, Brasil. Arch Zootec. 2005;509–13.
- 30. Hartman N, Greening LM. A Preliminary Study Investigating the Influence of Auditory Stimulation on the Occurrence of Nocturnal Equine Sleep-Related Behavior in Stabled Horses. J Equine Vet Sci. 2019;82:102782. pmid:31732116
- 31. Huo X, Wongkwanklom M, Phonraksa T, Na-Lampang P. Effects of playing classical music on behavior of stabled horses. VIS. 2020;19(2):259–67.
- 32. Houpt K, Marrow M, Seeliger M. A preliminary study of the effect of music on equine behavior. Journal of Equine Veterinary Science. 2000;20(11):691–737.
- 33. Fureix C, Jego P, Sankey C, Hausberger M. How horses (Equus caballus) see the world: humans as significant “objects”. Anim Cogn. 2009;12(4):643–54. pmid:19381698
- 34. Clark FE, Dunn JC. From Soundwave to Soundscape: A Guide to Acoustic Research in Captive Animal Environments. Front Vet Sci. 2022;9:889117. pmid:35782565
- 35. de Moraes RB. Estimativa de tons e acordes musicais mediante análise espectro-temporal. Rev Foco. 2023;16(02):e948.
- 36.
Nirmal MR, Mohan S. Music genre classification using spectrograms. In: 2020 International Conference on Power, Instrumentation, Control and Computing (PICC). 2020. p. 1–5. https://doi.org/10.1109/picc51425.2020.9362364
- 37.
Lima AR, Nanni L. Music genre recognition using spectrograms with harmonic-percussive sound separation. In: 35th International Conference of the Chilean Computer Science Society (SCCC). 2016. p. 1–7. https://doi.org/10.1109/sccc.2016.7836027
- 38. Lecoq L, Gains M, Blond L, Parent J. Brainstem auditory evoked responses in foals: reference values, effect of age, rate of acoustic stimulation, and neurologic deficits. J Vet Intern Med. 2015;29(1):362–7. pmid:25619523
- 39. McDuffee L, Mills M, McNiven M, Montelpare W. Establishing statistical stability for heart rate variability in horses. Journal of Veterinary Behavior. 2019;32:30–5.
- 40. Goodwin S. Comparison of Body Temperatures of Goats, Horses, and Sheep Measured With a Tympanic Infrared Thermometer, an Implantable Microchip Transponder, and a Rectal Thermometer. Contemp Top Lab Anim Sci. 1998;37(3):51–5. pmid:12456161
- 41. Jolly S, Art T, Lekeux P. Transient respiration locomotion decoupling in galloping Thoroughbreds. Equine Veterinary Journal. 1995;27(S18):13–7.
- 42. Jodkowska E, Dudek K, Soroko M. Temperature range analysis (Tmax) on dorsal surface of sporting horses. Turk J Vet Anim Sci. 2015;39:229–32.
- 43. Marlin DJ, Scott CM, Mills PC, Louwes H, Vaarten J. Rehydration following exercise: effects of administration of water versus an isotonic oral rehydration solution (ORS). Vet J. 1998;156(1):41–9. pmid:9691850
- 44.
Ekman P. Facial action coding system (FACS). A human face. 2002.
- 45. Friedman BH, Thayer JF. Anxiety and autonomic flexibility: a cardiovascular approach. Biol Psychol. 1998;47(3):243–63. pmid:9564452
- 46.
Acharya UR, Joseph KP, Kannathal N, Min LC, Suri JS. Heart rate variability. In: Advances in cardiac signal processing. 2007. p. 121–65.
- 47. Ohmura H, Jones JH. Changes in heart rate and heart rate variability as a function of age in Thoroughbred horses. J Equine Sci. 2017;28(3):99–103. pmid:28955161
- 48.
Feitosa FLF. Semiologia veterinária: a arte do diagnóstico. São Paulo: Roca; 2004.
- 49. Itao K, Komazawa M, Kobayashi H. A Study into Blood Flow, Heart Rate Variability, and Body Surface Temperature While Listening to Music. Health. 2018;10(02):181–8.
- 50. Ciborowska P, Michalczuk M, Bień D. The Effect of Music on Livestock: Cattle, Poultry and Pigs. Animals (Basel). 2021;11(12):3572. pmid:34944347
- 51. da Silva SAF, Guida HL, Dos SantosAntônio AM, Vanderlei LCM, Ferreira LL, de Abreu LC, et al. Auditory stimulation with music influences the geometric indices of heart rate variability in men. Int Arch Med. 2014;7:27. pmid:24883104
- 52.
Kemp A. The Effects of Music on Dairy Production. Murray State University Honors College: Murray, KY, USA. 2019.
- 53. Neveux C, Ferard M, Dickel L, Bouet V, Petit O, Valenchon M. Classical music reduces acute stress of domestic horses. Journal of Veterinary Behavior. 2016;15:81.
- 54. König v. Borstel U, Visser EK, Hall C. Indicators of stress in equitation. Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 2017;190:43–56.
- 55. Vinkers CH, Penning R, Hellhammer J, Verster JC, Klaessens JHGM, Olivier B, et al. The effect of stress on core and peripheral body temperature in humans. Stress. 2013;16(5):520–30. pmid:23790072
- 56. Kay R, Hall C. The use of a mirror reduces isolation stress in horses being transported by trailer. Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 2009;116(2–4):237–43.
- 57. Luzi F, Ludwig N, Gargano M, Milazzo M, Carenzi C, Verga M. Evaluation of skin temperature change as stress indicator in rabbit through infrared thermography. Italian Journal of Animal Science. 2007;6(sup1):769–769.
- 58.
Yarnell K. An investigation into the use of infrared thermography as a tool to assess the physiological stress response in the horse. United Kingdom: Nottingham Trent University; 2011.
- 59. McGreevy P, Warren-Smith A, Guisard Y. The effect of double bridles and jaw-clamping crank nosebands on temperature of eyes and facial skin of horses. Journal of Veterinary Behavior. 2012;7(3):142–8.
- 60. Valera M, Bartolomé E, Sánchez MJ, Molina A, Cook N, Schaefer A. Changes in Eye Temperature and Stress Assessment in Horses During Show Jumping Competitions. Journal of Equine Veterinary Science. 2012;32(12):827–30.
- 61. Bartolomé E, Sánchez MJ, Molina A, Schaefer AL, Cervantes I, Valera M. Using eye temperature and heart rate for stress assessment in young horses competing in jumping competitions and its possible influence on sport performance. Animal. 2013;7(12):2044–53. pmid:24067493
- 62. Stewart M, Stafford KJ, Dowling SK, Schaefer AL, Webster JR. Eye temperature and heart rate variability of calves disbudded with or without local anaesthetic. Physiol Behav. 2008;93(4–5):789–97. pmid:18177678
- 63. Cook N, Schaefer A, Warren L, Burwash L, Anderson M, Baron V. Adrenocortical and metabolic responses to acth injection in horses: an assessment by salivary cortisol and infrared thermography of the eye. Can J Anim Sci. 2001;81:621.
- 64. Yarnell K, Hall C, Billett E. An assessment of the aversive nature of an animal management procedure (clipping) using behavioral and physiological measures. Physiol Behav. 2013;118:32–9. pmid:23685232
- 65. Tomberg C, Petagna M, de Selliers de Moranville L-A. Horses (Equus caballus) facial micro-expressions: insight into discreet social information. Sci Rep. 2023;13(1):8625. pmid:37244937
- 66. Mendl M, Burman OHP, Paul ES. An integrative and functional framework for the study of animal emotion and mood. Proc Biol Sci. 2010;277(1696):2895–904. pmid:20685706
- 67. Lundblad J, Rashid M, Rhodin M, Andersen PH. Facial expressions of emotional stress in horses. Biorxiv. 2020;10.
- 68. Rashid M, Silventoinen A, Gleerup KB, Andersen PH. Equine Facial Action Coding System for determination of pain-related facial responses in videos of horses. PLoS One. 2020;15(11):e0231608. pmid:33141852
- 69. Kovac M, Vladimirovna Ippolitova T, Pozyabin S, Aliev R, Lobanova V, Drakul N, et al. Equine Stress: Neuroendocrine Physiology and Pathophysiology. Veterinary Medicine and Science. IntechOpen. 2022.
- 70. Carmo LG, Werner LC, Michelotto PV, Daros RR. Horse behavior and facial movements in relation to food rewards. PLoS ONE. 2023;18(6):e0286045.
- 71. Lemasson A, Remeuf K, Trabalon M, Cuir F, Hausberger M. Mares prefer the voices of highly fertile stallions. PLoS One. 2015;10(2):e0118468. pmid:25714814
- 72. Lindig AM, McGreevy PD, Crean AJ. Musical Dogs: A Review of the Influence of Auditory Enrichment on Canine Health and Behavior. Animals (Basel). 2020;10(1):127. pmid:31941064
- 73. Nater UM, Abbruzzese E, Krebs M, Ehlert U. Sex differences in emotional and psychophysiological responses to musical stimuli. Int J Psychophysiol. 2006;62(2):300–8. pmid:16828911
- 74. Williams JE. Gender-related worry and emotionality test anxiety for high-achieving students. Psychol Schs. 1998;33(2):159–62.
- 75. Barton A, Lindenberg I, Einspanier A, Merle R, Gehlen H. Evaluation of the effect of tongue ties on stress parameters, behaviour and heart-rate variability in racehorses. Anim welf. 2022;31(2):231–41.